全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Cognition is … Fundamentally Cultural

DOI: 10.3390/bs3010042

Keywords: cognition, culture, computer metaphor, numerical cognition, ethnobiological reasoning, theory of mind

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

A prevailing concept of cognition in psychology is inspired by the computer metaphor. Its focus on mental states that are generated and altered by information input, processing, storage and transmission invites a disregard for the cultural dimension of cognition, based on three (implicit) assumptions: cognition is internal, processing can be distinguished from content, and processing is independent of cultural background. Arguing against each of these assumptions, we point out how culture may affect cognitive processes in various ways, drawing on instances from numerical cognition, ethnobiological reasoning, and theory of mind. Given the pervasive cultural modulation of cognition—on all of Marr’s levels of description—we conclude that cognition is indeed fundamentally cultural, and that consideration of its cultural dimension is essential for a comprehensive understanding.

References

[1]  Norman, D.A. Twelve issues for cognitive science. Cogn. Sci. 1980, 4, 1–32, doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0401_1.
[2]  If you enter, for instance, “causal cognition” as title, abstract, subjects, or keywords into PsychINFO, the database of the APA, you obtain 59 hits (20 November 2012). If you combine these with “culture” or “cultural” in the same categories, the selection narrows down to four, two of which are not on causal cognition in the strict sense, but on motivation and morality, respectively.
[3]  Arnett, J.J. The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. Am. Psychol. 2008, 63, 602–614, doi:10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602.
[4]  Henrich, J.; Heine, S.J.; Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 61–135, doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.
[5]  Norenzayan, A.; Heine, S.J. Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychol. Bull. 2005, 131, 763–784, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.763.
[6]  Neisser, U. Cognitive Psychology; Appleton-Century-Crofts: East Norwalk, CT, USA, 1967.
[7]  Medin, D.L.; Ross, B.H.; Markman, A.B. Cognitive Psychology; John Wiley and Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2004.
[8]  Beller, S.; Bender, A. Allgemeine Psychologie: Denken und Sprache; Hogrefe: G?ttingen, Germany, 2010.
[9]  Boden, M.A. Mind as Machine; Clarendon: Oxford, UK, 2006.
[10]  Gardner, H. The Mind’s New Science; Basic: New York, NY, USA, 1985.
[11]  Miller, G.A. The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 141–144, doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9.
[12]  Quinlan, P.; Dyson, B. Cognitive Psychology; Pearson Education: Essex, UK, 2008.
[13]  Appraisal Processes in Emotion; Scherer, K.R., Schorr, A., Johnstone, T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2001.
[14]  Emotions and Beliefs; Frijda, N.H., Manstead, A.S.R., Bem, S., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000.
[15]  Kunda, Z. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 1990, 108, 480–498, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480.
[16]  Holyoak, K.J. Psychology. In The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences; Wilson, R.A., Keil, F.C., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999; pp. xxxviiii–xlix.
[17]  This is obvious for the cold cognitions as listed above, for instance when we ponder on a task and then communicate the result. The same holds for the hot cognition components of emotion and motivation, such as when we perceive a situation in a fraction of a second, appraise it as threatening, and respond to it with a specific emotion (in this case very likely with fear), which, in turn, triggers an action tendency.
[18]  Marr, D. Vision; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1981.
[19]  Willems, R.M. Re-appreciating the why of cognition: 35 years after Marr and Poggio. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 244.
[20]  Bender, A.; Beller, S. The cultural constitution of cognition: Taking the anthropological perspective. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 67.
[21]  Norman, D.A. Things That Make Us Smart; Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA, USA, 1993.
[22]  Hutchins, E. Cognition in the Wild; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995.
[23]  Hutchins, E. Cognitive ecology. Topics Cogn. Sci. 2010, 2, 705–715.
[24]  Nickerson, R.S. Counting, computing, and the representation of number. Hum. Factors 1988, 30, 181–199.
[25]  Zhang, J.; Norman, D.A. A representational analysis of numeration systems. Cognition 1995, 57, 271–295, doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00674-3.
[26]  Zhang, J.; Wang, H. The effect of external representations on numeric tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2005, 58, 817–838, doi:10.1080/02724980443000340.
[27]  Gibson, J.J. The ecological approach to visual perception; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. This is related to the Gibsonian idea that artifacts “afford” and thus not only facilitate, but invite certain behavioral responses..
[28]  Di Luca, S.; Pesenti, M. Finger numeral representations: More than just another symbolic code. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 272.
[29]  Hurford, J.R. Language and Number; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 1987.
[30]  Wiese, H. Numbers, Language, and the Human Mind; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2003.
[31]  Chrisomalis, S. A cognitive typology for numerical notation. Cambr. Arch. J. 2004, 14, 37–52, doi:10.1017/S0959774304000034.
[32]  Chrisomalis, S. Numerical Notation; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010.
[33]  Schlimm, D.; Neth, H. Modeling ancient and modern arithmetic practices: Addition and multiplication with Arabic and Roman numerals. In Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society; Love, B.C., McRae, K., Sloutsky, V.M., Eds.; Cognitive Science Society: Austin, TX, USA, 2008; pp. 2097–2102.
[34]  Widom, T.R.; Schlimm, D. Methodological reflections on typologies for numerical notations. Sci. Context 2012, 25, 155–195, doi:10.1017/S0269889712000038.
[35]  Bender, A.; Beller, S. Fingers as a tool for counting—Naturally fixed or culturally flexible? Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 256.
[36]  Bender, A.; Beller, S. Numerical cognition and ethnomathematics. In A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology; Kronenfeld, D., Bennardo, G., de Munck, V.C., Fischer, M., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell: Chichester, UK, 2011; pp. 270–289.
[37]  Bender, A.; Beller, S. Nature and culture of finger counting: Diversity and representational effects of an embodied cognitive tool. Cognition 2012, 124, 156–182, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.005.
[38]  Beller, S.; Bender, A. Explicating numerical information: When and how fingers support (or hinder) number comprehension and handling. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 214.
[39]  Lordahl, D.S.; Krop, H.; Jacobson, L.I. Information processing using number systems with bases higher than ten. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Beh. 1970, 9, 155–160, doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80044-5.
[40]  Domahs, F.; Moeller, K.; Huber, S.; Willmes, K.; Nuerk, H.-C. Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition 2010, 116, 251–266, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007.
[41]  Krajcsi, A.; Szabo, E. The role of number notation: Sign-value notation number processing is easier than place-value. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 463.
[42]  Byrne, R.M.J. Suppressing valid inferences with conditionals? Cognition 1989, 31, 61–83, doi:10.1016/0010-0277(89)90018-8.
[43]  Beller, S.; Spada, H. The logic of content effects in propositional reasoning: The case of conditional reasoning with a point of view. Think. Reas. 2003, 9, 335–378, doi:10.1080/13546780342000007.
[44]  Klauer, K.C.; Beller, S.; Hütter, M. Conditional reasoning in context: A dual-source model of probabilistic inference. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. 2010, 36, 298–323, doi:10.1037/a0018705.
[45]  Beller, S. Concrete problems in the abstract deontic selection task—And how to solve them. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 2012, 65, 1414–1429, doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.656667.
[46]  In this approach, psychology differs fundamentally, for instance, from cognitive anthropology, which is primarily interested in the concrete (potentially culturally specific) content of mental representations, i.e., in the cultural knowledge of groups of persons, on how this knowledge is organized and described, transmitted and modified [121]. The division of labor between cognitive anthropology and psychology [120] seemed justified by the assumed separability of process and content, but has been criticized more recently, not only because it contributed to the alienation of the two disciplines [20,125], but, more importantly, because the distinction itself turned out to be neither reasonable nor tenable [48,60].
[47]  Mehlhorn, K.; Sanders, P. Algorithms and Data Structures; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2008.
[48]  Bang, M.; Medin, D.L.; Atran, S. Cultural mosaics and mental models of nature. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2007, 104, 13868–13874.
[49]  Heit, E. Properties of inductive reasoning. Psychol. Bull. Rev. 2000, 7, 569–592, doi:10.3758/BF03212996.
[50]  Murphy, G.L. The Big Book of Concepts; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2002.
[51]  Osherson, D.N.; Wilkie, O.; Smith, E.E.; López, A.; Shafir, E. Category-based induction. Psychol. Rev. 1990, 97, 185–200, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.185.
[52]  Rosch, E.H. Cognitive representations of semantic categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1975, 104, 192–233, doi:10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192.
[53]  Sloman, S.A. Feature-based induction. Cognitive Psych. 1993, 25, 231–280, doi:10.1006/cogp.1993.1006.
[54]  López, A.; Atran, S.; Coley, J.D.; Medin, D.L.; Smith, E.E. The tree of life: Universal and cultural features of folkbiological taxonomies and inductions. Cogn. Psychol. 1997, 32, 251–295, doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0651.
[55]  Bailenson, J.N.; Shum, M.S.; Atran, S.; Medin, D.L.; Coley, J.D. A bird’s eye view: Biological categorization and reasoning within and across cultures. Cognition 2002, 84, 1–53, doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00011-2.
[56]  Recent approaches now take this into consideration, e.g., Tenenbaum, J.B.; Griffiths, T.L.; Kemp, C. Theory-based Bayesian models of inductive learning and reasoning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 309–318
[57]  Atran, S.; Medin, D.L. The Native Mind and the Cultural Construction of Nature; MIT Press: Boston, MA, USA, 2008.
[58]  Berlin, B. Ethnobiological Classification; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1992.
[59]  Ethnobiology and the Science of Humankind; Ellen, R., Ed.; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2006.
[60]  Medin, D.L.; Atran, S. The native mind: Biological categorization, reasoning and decision making in development across cultures. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 111, 960–983, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960.
[61]  The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise Performance; Ericsson, K.A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P.J., Hoffman, R.R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006.
[62]  Wilson, M. The re-tooled mind: How culture re-engineers cognition. SCAN 2010, 5, 180–187.
[63]  Maguire, E.A.; Gadian, D.G.; Johnsrude, I.S.; Good, C.D.; Ashburner, J.; Frackowiak, R.S.J.; Frith, C.D. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 4398–4403.
[64]  Carreiras, M.; Seghier, M.L.; Baquero, S.; Estévez, A.; Lozano, A.; Devlin, J.T.; Price, C.J. An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature 2009, 461, 983–986, doi:10.1038/nature08461.
[65]  Petersson, K.M.; Silva, C.; Alexandre Castro-Caldas, A.; Ingvar, M.; Reis, A. Literacy: A cultural influence on functional left-right differences in the inferior parietal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007, 26, 791–799, doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05701.x.
[66]  Levinson, S.C. The original sin of cognitive science. Topics Cogn. Sci. 2012, 4, 396–403, doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01195.x.
[67]  Carey, S. On the origins of causal understanding. In Causal Cognition; Sperber, D., Premack, D., Premack, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 268–308.
[68]  Carey, S. The Origin of Concepts; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009.
[69]  Atran, S.; Medin, D.L.; Lynch, E.; Vapnarsky, V.; Ucan Ek’, E.; Sousa, P. Folkbiology doesn’t come from folkpsychology: Evidence from Yukatek Maya in cross-cultural perspective. J. Cogn. Cult. 2001, 1, 3–42, doi:10.1163/156853701300063561.
[70]  Evans, N.; Levinson, S.C. The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 2009, 32, 429–492, doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999094X.
[71]  Segall, M.H.; Campbell, D.T.; Herskovits, M.J. Cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions. Science 1963, 139, 769–771.
[72]  Ahluwalia, A. An intra-cultural investigation of susceptibility to ‘perspective’ and ‘non-perspective’ spatial illusions. Br. J. Psychol. 1978, 69, 233–241, doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1978.tb01653.x.
[73]  Chan, T.T.; Bergen, B. Writing direction influences spatial cognition. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society; Bara, B., Barsalou, L.W., Bucciarelli, M., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 412–417.
[74]  Masuda, T.; Nisbett, R.E. Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 81, 922–934, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922.
[75]  Masuda, T.; Nisbett, R.E. Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Sci. 2006, 30, 381–399, doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_63.
[76]  Majid, A.; Bowerman, M.; Kita, S.; Haun, D.B.M.; Levinson, S.C. Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends Cognitive Sci. 2004, 8, 108–114, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.003.
[77]  Haun, D.B.M.; Rapold, C.J.; Call, J.; Janzen, G.; Levinson, S.C. Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 17568–17573.
[78]  Haun, D.B.M.; Rapold, C.J.; Janzen, G.; Levinson, S.C. Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cognition covary across cultures. Cognition 2011, 119, 70–80, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.009.
[79]  Bender, A.; Beller, S.; Bennardo, G. Temporal frames of reference: Conceptual analysis and empirical evidence from German, English, Mandarin Chinese, and Tongan. J. Cogn. Cult. 2010, 10, 283–307, doi:10.1163/156853710X531195.
[80]  Beller, S.; Rothe-Wulf, A.; Hüther, L.; Bender, A. Moving forward in space and time: How strong is the conceptual link between spatial and temporal frames of reference? Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 486.
[81]  Boroditsky, L.; Gaby, A. Remembrances of times East: Absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian Aboriginal community. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 1635–1639, doi:10.1177/0956797610386621.
[82]  Le Guen, O.; Pool Balam, L.I. No metaphorical timeline in gesture and cognition among Yucatec Mayas. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 271.
[83]  Nú?ez, R.E.; Cooperrider, K.; Doan, D.; Wassmann, J. Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition 2012, 124, 25–35, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.007.
[84]  Beller, S.; Bender, A. The limits of counting: Numerical cognition between evolution and culture. Science 2008, 319, 213–215, doi:10.1126/science.1148345.
[85]  Pica, P.; Lemer, C.; Izard, V.; Dehaene, S. Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 2004, 306, 499–503, doi:10.1126/science.1102085.
[86]  Miller, K.F.; Smith, C.M.; Zhu, J.; Zhang, H. Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychol. Sci. 1995, 6, 56–60, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00305.x.
[87]  Norenzayan, A.; Nisbett, R.E. Culture and causal cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Res. 2000, 9, 132–135.
[88]  Beller, S.; Bender, A.; Song, J. Weighing up physical causes: Effects of culture, linguistic cues and content. J. Cogn. Cult. 2009, 9, 347–365, doi:10.1163/156770909X12518536414493.
[89]  Bender, A.; Beller, S. Causal asymmetry across cultures: Assigning causal roles in symmetric physical settings. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 231.
[90]  Wellman, H.M.; Cross, D.; Watson, J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 655–684.
[91]  Lillard, A. Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 123, 3–46, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.3.
[92]  Vinden, P.G. Junín Quechua children’s understanding of mind. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 1707–1716, doi:10.2307/1131726.
[93]  Callaghan, T.; Rochat, P.; Lillard, A.; Claux, M.L.; Odden, H.; Itakura, S.; Tapanya, S.; Singh, S. Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 378–384, doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01544.x.
[94]  Liu, D.; Wellman, H.M.; Tardif, T.; Sabbagh, M.A. Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 523–531, doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523.
[95]  Mayer, A.; Tr?uble, B. Synchrony in the onset of mental state understanding across cultures? A study among children in Samoa. Int. J. Beh. Dev. 2012.
[96]  de Villiers, J.G.; Pyers, J.E. Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cogn. Dev. 2002, 17, 1037–1060, doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4.
[97]  Tardif, T.; Wellman, H.M.; Cheung, K.M. False belief understanding in Cantonese-speaking children. J. Child Lang. 2004, 31, 779–800.
[98]  Woolfe, T.; Want, S.C.; Siegal, M. Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 768–778, doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00437.
[99]  Gauvain, M. Culture, development, and theory of mind: Comment on Lillard (1998). Psychol. Bull. 1998, 123, 37–42, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.37.
[100]  Robbins, J.; Rumsey, A. Introduction: Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropol. Quart. 2008, 81, 407–420, doi:10.1353/anq.0.0005.
[101]  Theory of Mind: Reasoning Across Cultures; Wassmann, J., Tr?uble, B., Funke, J., Eds.; Berghahn: New York, NY, USA. in press..
[102]  Whorf, B.L. Language, Thought and Reality; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1956.
[103]  Hunt, E.; Agnoli, F. The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychology perspective. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 98, 377–389, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.377.
[104]  Lee, P. The Whorf Theory Complex: A Critical Reconstruction; Benjamins: Amsterdam, NL, USA, 1996.
[105]  Lucy, J.A. Language Diversity and Thought; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1992.
[106]  Lucy, J.A. Linguistic relativity. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 1997, 26, 291–312, doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.26.1.291.
[107]  Rethinking Linguistic Relativity; Gumperz, J.J., Levinson, S.C., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996.
[108]  Levinson, S.C.; Kita, S.; Haun, D.B.M.; Rasch, B.H. Returning the tables: Language affects spatial reasoning. Cognition 2002, 84, 155–188, doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00045-8.
[109]  Pinker, S. The Language Instinct; Perennial: New York, NY, USA, 1995.
[110]  Li, P.; Gleitman, L. Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning. Cognition 2002, 83, 265–294, doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00009-4.
[111]  Gleitman, L.R.; Papafragou, A. Language and thought. In The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning; Morrison, R., Holyoak, K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 633–661.
[112]  Li, P.; Abarbanell, L.; Papafragou, A.; Gleitman, L. Spatial reasoning in Tenejapan Mayans. Cognition 2011, 120, 33–53, doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.012.
[113]  Language in Mind: Advances in the Investigation of Language and Thought; Gentner, D., Goldin-Meadow, S., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003.
[114]  Regier, T.; Kay, P. Language, thought, and color: Whorf was half right. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 439–446, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.001.
[115]  Stenning, K.; van Lambalgen, M. Human Reasoning and Cognitive Science; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008.
[116]  Stenning, K. To naturalize or not to naturalize? An issue for cognitive science as well as anthropology. Topics Cogn. Sci. 2012, 4, 413–419, doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01200.x.
[117]  Kitayama, S.; Uskul, A. Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 419–449, doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357.
[118]  Tomasello, M. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999.
[119]  Bloch, M. How We Think They Think; Westview: Oxford, UK, 1998.
[120]  D’Andrade, R.G. The cultural part of cognition. Cogn. Sci. 1981, 5, 179–195.
[121]  D’Andrade, R.G. The Development of Cognitive Anthropology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995.
[122]  Kronenfeld, D.B. Culture, Society, and Cognition; Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2008.
[123]  Shore, B. Culture in Mind; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996.
[124]  Shweder, R.A. Thinking through Cultures; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1991.
[125]  Bender, A.; Hutchins, E.; Medin, D.L. Anthropology in cognitive science. Topics Cogn. Sci. 2010, 2, 374–385, doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01082.x.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413