全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Response to Hypothetical Social Scenarios in Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury Who Present Inappropriate Social Behavior: A Preliminary Report

DOI: 10.3390/bs3010072

Keywords: inappropriate social behavior, traumatic brain injury, decision making, social interaction

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Background: Very little research thus far has examined the decision making that underlies inappropriate social behavior (ISB) post-TBI (traumatic brain injury). Objectives: To verify the usefulness of a new instrument, the Social Responding Task, for investigating whether, in social decision making, individuals with TBI, who present inappropriate social behavior (ISB), have difficulty anticipating their own feelings of embarrassment and others’ angry reactions following an ISB. Methods: Seven subjects with TBI presenting with inappropriate social behavior (TBI-ISB), 10 presenting with appropriate social behavior (TBI-ASB), and 15 healthy controls were given 12 hypothetical scenarios three times, each time ending with a different behavioral response. Subjects were asked to gauge the likelihood of their displaying the behavior in that situation (part A) and of it being followed by an angry reaction from the other or by feelings of embarrassment in themselves (part B). Results: TBI-ISB subjects scored higher than TBI-ASB and healthy controls on a scale of likelihood of displaying an ISB. Results regarding expectations of angry reactions from others and feelings of embarrassment after an ISB were similar among groups. Negative correlations between endorsement of an inappropriate behavior and anticipation of negative emotional consequences were significant for both TBI-ASB and control subjects, but not for TBI-ISB subjects. Conclusions: Results suggest that the TBI-ISB participants were likely to endorse an ISB despite being able to anticipate a negative emotional response in themselves or others, suggesting that there were other explanations for their poor behavior. A self-reported likely response to hypothetical social scenarios can be a useful approach for studying the neurocognitive processes behind the poor choices of individuals with TBI-ISB, but the task needs further validation studies. A comprehensive discussion follows on the underlying mechanisms affecting social behaviors after a TBI.

References

[1]  Damasio, A.R.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, H. Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 1990, 41, 81–94, doi:10.1016/0166-4328(90)90144-4.
[2]  Eslinger, P.J.; Damasio, A.R. Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology 1985, 35, 1731–1741, doi:10.1212/WNL.35.12.1731.
[3]  Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R. Emotion, Decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebr. Cortex 2000, 10, 295–307, doi:10.1093/cercor/10.3.295.
[4]  Damasio, A.R. Descartes’s error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain; Grosset/Putnam: New York, NY, USA, 1994.
[5]  Leland, J.W.; Grafman, J. Experimental tests of the somatic marker hypothesis. Game. Econ. Behav. 2005, 52, 386–409, doi:10.1016/j.geb.2004.09.001.
[6]  Bechara, A.; Damasio, A.R.; Damasio, H.; Anderson, S.W. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994, 50, 7–15, doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3.
[7]  Bechara, A.; Damasio, A.R.; Tranel, D.; Anderson, S.W. Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 1998, 18, 428–437.
[8]  Bechara, A. The role of emotion in decision making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognit. 2004, 55, 30–40, doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001.
[9]  Camille, N.; Coricelli, G.; Sallet, J.; Pradat-Diehl, P.; Duhamel, J.-R.; Sirigu, A. The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science 2004, 304, 1167–1170, doi:10.1126/science.1094550.
[10]  Krajbich, I.; Adolphs, R.; Tranel, D.; Denburg, N.L.; Camerer, C.F. Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 2188–2192, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5086-08.2009.
[11]  Koenigs, M.; Tranel, D. Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum game. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 951–956, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4606-06.2007.
[12]  Bigler, E.D. The lesion(s) in traumatic brain injury: Implications for clinical neuropsychology. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2001, 16, 95–131.
[13]  Brooks, D.N.; McKinlay, W. Personality and behavioural change after severe blunt head injury—a relative's view. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 1983, 46, 336–344, doi:10.1136/jnnp.46.4.336.
[14]  Thomsen, I.V. Late outcome of very severe blunt head trauma: A 10-15 year second follow up. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 1984, 47, 260–268, doi:10.1136/jnnp.47.3.260.
[15]  Franulic, A.; Horta, E.; Maturana, R.; Scherpenisse, J.; Carbonell, C. Organic personality disorder after traumatic brain injury: Cognitive, anatomic and psychosocial factors. A 6 month follow up. Brain Inj. 2000, 14, 431–439, doi:10.1080/026990500120538.
[16]  Lezak, M.D. Relationships between personality disorders, social disturbances, and physical disability following traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 1987, 2, 57–69, doi:10.1097/00001199-198703000-00009.
[17]  Lezak, M.D. Living with the characterologically altered brain injured patient. J. Clin. Psychiatr. 1978, 39, 592–598.
[18]  Kelly, G.; Brown, S.; Todd, J.; Kremer, P.; Kelly, G.; Brown, S.; Todd, J.; Kremer, P. Challenging behaviour profiles of people with acquired brain injury living in community settings. Brain Inj. 2008, 22, 457–470, doi:10.1080/02699050802060647.
[19]  Levine, B.; Black, S.E.; Cheung, G.; Campbell, A.; O’Toole, C.; Schwartz, M.L. Gambling task performance in traumatic brain injury: Relationships to injury severity, atrophy, lesion location, and cognitive and psychosocial outcome. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2005, 18, 45–54, doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000152227.13001.c3.
[20]  Wiederkehr, S.; Barat, M.; Dehail, P.; de Sèze, M.; Loze-Boudillon, S.; Giroire, J.M. Decision making and executive function in severe traumatic brain injured patients: Validation of a decision-making task and correlated features. Revue de Neurologie (Paris) 2005, 161, 201–210, doi:10.1016/S0035-3787(05)85023-4.
[21]  Bonatti, E.; Zamarian, L.; Wagner, M.; Benke, T.; Hollosi, P.; Strubreither, W.; Delazer, M. Making decisions and advising decisions in traumatic brain injury. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2008, 21, 164–175, doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e318184e688.
[22]  Milders, M.; Fuchs, S.; Crawford, J.R. Neuropsychological impairments and changes in emotional and social behaviour following severe traumatic brain injury. J.Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2003, 25, 157–172, doi:10.1076/jcen.25.2.157.13642.
[23]  Rochat, L.; Ammann, J.; Mayer, E.; Annoni, J.-M.; van der Linden, M. Executive disorders and perceived socio-emotional changes after traumatic brain injury. J. Neuropsychol. 2009, 3, 213–227, doi:10.1348/174866408X397656.
[24]  Stone, V.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Knight, R. Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1998, 10, 640–656, doi:10.1162/089892998562942.
[25]  Blair, R.J.; Cipolotti, L. Impaired social response reversal: A case of ‘acquired sociopathy’. Brain 2000, 123, 1122–1141, doi:10.1093/brain/123.6.1122.
[26]  Pettit, G.S.; Dodge, K.A.; Brown, M.M. Early family experience, Social problem solving patterns, And children’s social competence. Child Dev. 1988, 59, 107–120, doi:10.2307/1130393.
[27]  Kelly, G.; Todd, J.; Simpson, G.; Kremer, P.; Martin, C.; Kelly, G.; Todd, J.; Simpson, G.; Kremer, P.; Martin, C. The overt behaviour scale (obs): A tool for measuring challenging behaviours following abi in community settings. Brain Inj. 2006, 20, 307–319, doi:10.1080/02699050500488074.
[28]  Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed.; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1997.
[29]  Rabin, L.A.; Barr, W.B.; Burton, L.A. Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the united states and canada: A survey of ins, nan, and apa division 40 members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2005, 20, 33–65, doi:10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005.
[30]  Rochat, L.; Beni, C.; Billieux, J.; Azouvi, P.; Annoni, J.-M.; Van der Linden, M. Assessment of impulsivity after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neuopsychol. Rehabil. 2010, 20, 778–797, doi:10.1080/09602011.2010.495245.
[31]  Whiteside, S.P.; Lynam, D.R. The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2001, 30, 669–689, doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7.
[32]  Reynolds, W.M. Development of reliable and valid short forms of the marlowe-crowne social desirability scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982, 38, 119–125, doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1<119::AID-JCLP2270380118>3.0.CO;2-I.
[33]  Gravetter, F.J.; Wallnau, L.B. Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences; Wadsworth: Belmont, CA, USA, 2004.
[34]  Bechara, A.; Van der Linden, M. Decision-making and impulse control after frontal lobe injuries. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2005, 18, 734–739, doi:10.1097/01.wco.0000194141.56429.3c.
[35]  Fontaine, R.G.; Dodge, K.A. Real-time decision making and aggressive behavior in youth: A heuristic model of response evaluation and decision (RED). Aggress. Behav. 2006, 32, 604–624, doi:10.1002/ab.20150.
[36]  Billieux, J.; Gay, P.; Rochat, L.; Van der Linden, M. The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms in problematic behaviours. Behav. Res. Ther. 2010, 48, 1085–1096, doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.008.
[37]  Franzen, M.D. Neuropsychological assessment in traumatic brain injury. Crit. Care. Nurs. Q 2000, 23, 58–64.
[38]  Grafman, J.; Schwab, K.; Warden, D.; Pridgen, B.S.; Brown, H.R.; Salazar, A.M. Frontal lobe injuries, Violence, And aggression: A report of the vietnam head injury study. Neurology 1996, 46, 1231–1238, doi:10.1212/WNL.46.5.1231.
[39]  Floden, D.; Alexander, M.P.; Kubu, C.S.; Katz, D.; Stuss, D.T. Impulsivity and risk-taking behavior in focal frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 213–223, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.020.
[40]  Barrash, J.; Tranel, D.; Anderson, S.W. Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2000, 18, 355–381, doi:10.1207/S1532694205Barrash.
[41]  Anderson, S.W.; Damasio, H.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, A.R. Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2000, 18, 281–296, doi:10.1207/S1532694202Anderson.
[42]  Crick, N.R.; Dodge, K.A. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's development. Psychol. Bull. 1994, 115, 74–101, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74.
[43]  Shallice, T.; Burgess, P.W.; Schon, F.; Baxter, D.M. The origins of utilization behavior. Brain 1989, 112, 1587–1598, doi:10.1093/brain/112.6.1587.
[44]  Cicerone, K.D.; Tanenbaum, L.N. Disturbance of social cognition after traumatic orbitofrontal brain injury. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1997, 12, 173–188.
[45]  Burgess, P.W. Strategy application disorder: The role of the frontal lobes in human multitasking. Psychol. Res. 2000, 63, 279–288, doi:10.1007/s004269900006.
[46]  Stuss, D.T.; Shallice, T.; Alexander, M.P.; Picton, T.W. A multidisciplinary approach to anterior attentional functions. Ann. New York Acad. Sci. 1995, 769, 191–211, doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38140.x.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413