全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Observed Human Actions, and Not Mechanical Actions, Induce Searching Errors in Infants

DOI: 10.1155/2012/465458

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Recent neurophysiological studies have shown that several human brain regions involved in executing actions are activated by merely observing such actions via a human, and not by a mechanical hand. At a behavioral level, observing a human’s movements, but not those of a robot, significantly interferes with ongoing executed movements. However, it is unclear whether the biological tuning in the observation/execution matching system are functional during infancy. The present study examines whether a human’s actions, and not a mechanical action, influence infants’ execution of the same actions due to the observation/execution matching system. Twelve-month-old infants were given a searching task. In the tasks, infants observed an object hidden at location A, after which either a human hand (human condition) or a mechanical one (mechanical condition) searched the object correctly. Next, the object was hidden at location B and infants were allowed to search the object. We examined whether infants searched the object at location B correctly. The results revealed that infants in the human condition were more likely to search location A than those in the mechanical condition. Moreover, the results suggested that infants’ searching behaviors were affected by their observations of the same actions by a human, but not a mechanical hand. Thus, it may be concluded that the observation/execution matching system may be biologically tuned during infancy. 1. Introduction It has been proposed that actions are intrinsically linked to perception, and that imagining, observing, or in any way representing an action, excites the motor programs used to execute such actions. This proposal is originally derived from James’ ideomotor theory [1] and recently developed by Prinz’s common coding framework [2]. According to this framework, the representation of a perceived action involves simulative production of that action on the part of the observer. This covert motor activation results in the observation of an action facilitating its execution. Neurophysiological studies have supported the common coding framework by showing that several brain regions (e.g., the primary motor cortex) involved in executing actions are activated by the mere observation of such actions, through what is known as the mirror neuron in animals [3, 4] and the mirror neuron system in human [5, 6]. Importantly, the mirror neuron system can respond only to biological actions [7]. On the behavioral level, the biological tuning leads to the fact that observing a human’s arm movements, but not those of a robot,

References

[1]  W. James, Principles of Psychology, Holt, New York, NY, USA, 1890.
[2]  W. Prinz, “A common coding approach to perception and action,” in Relationships Between Perception and Action, O. Neumann and W. Prinz, Eds., pp. 167–201, Springer, Heidelberg, Germany, 1990.
[3]  V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti, “Action recognition in the premotor cortex,” Brain, vol. 119, no. 2, pp. 593–609, 1996.
[4]  G. Rizzolatti, L. Fadiga, V. Gallese, and L. Fogassi, “Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 131–141, 1996.
[5]  G. Buccino, F. Binkofski, G. R. Fink et al., “Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study,” European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 400–404, 2001.
[6]  J. Decety, J. Grèzes, N. Costes et al., “Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy,” Brain, vol. 120, no. 10, pp. 1763–1777, 1997.
[7]  Y. F. Tai, C. Scherfler, D. J. Brooks, N. Sawamoto, and U. Castiello, “The human premotor cortex Is 'mirror' only for biological actions,” Current Biology, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 117–120, 2004.
[8]  C. Press, G. Bird, R. Flach, and C. Heyes, “Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 632–640, 2005.
[9]  J. M. Kilner, Y. Paulignan, and S. J. Blakemore, “An interference effect of observed biological movement on action,” Current Biology, vol. 13, no. 6, pp. 522–525, 2003.
[10]  G. Rizzolatti, L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese, “Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 9, pp. 661–670, 2001.
[11]  S. Shimada and K. Hiraki, “Infant's brain responses to live and televised action,” NeuroImage, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 930–939, 2006.
[12]  T. Falck-Ytter, G. Gredeb?ck, and C. von Hofsten, “Infants predict other people's action goals,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 9, no. 7, pp. 878–879, 2006.
[13]  Y. Moriguchi, T. Kanda, H. Ishiguro, and S. Itakura, “Children perseverate to a human's actions but not to a robot's actions,” Developmental Science, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 62–68, 2010.
[14]  A. N. Meltzoff, “Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 31, no. 5, pp. 838–850, 1995.
[15]  Y. Kanakogi and S. Itakura, “Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy,” Nature Communications, vol. 2, no. 1, article 341, 2011.
[16]  T. W. Boyer, J. Samantha Pan, and B. I. Bertenthal, “Infants' understanding of actions performed by mechanical devices,” Cognition, vol. 121, no. 1, pp. 1–11, 2011.
[17]  M. R. Longo and B. I. Bertenthal, “Common coding of observation and execution of action in 9-month-old infants,” Infancy, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 43–59, 2006.
[18]  J. Piaget, The Construction of Reality in the Child, Basic Books, New York, NY, USA, 1954.
[19]  Y. Munakata, J. L. McClelland, M. H. Johnson, and R. S. Siegler, “Rethinking infant knowledge: toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks,” Psychological Review, vol. 104, no. 4, pp. 686–713, 1997.
[20]  H. M. Wellman, D. Cross, and K. Bartsch, “Infant search and object permanence: a meta-analysis of the A-not-B error,” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol. 51, no. 3, pp. 1–67, 1986.
[21]  E. Thelen, G. Sch?ner, C. Scheier, and L. B. Smith, “The dynamics of embodiment: a field theory of infant perseverative reaching,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 24, no. 1, pp. 1–34, 2001.
[22]  S. Marcovitch and P. D. Zelazo, “A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function,” Developmental Science, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 1–25, 2009.
[23]  L. B. Smith, E. Thelen, R. Titzer, and D. McLin, “Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error,” Psychological Review, vol. 106, no. 2, pp. 235–260, 1999.
[24]  A. Diamond, “Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development,” in The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition, S. Carey and R. Gelman, Eds., pp. 67–110, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991.
[25]  J. A. Sommerville, A. L. Woodward, and A. Needham, “Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions,” Cognition, vol. 96, no. 1, pp. B1–B11, 2005.
[26]  V. Southgate, M. H. Johnson, T. Osborne, and G. Csibra, “Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants,” Biology Letters, vol. 5, no. 6, pp. 769–772, 2009.
[27]  A. L. Woodward, “Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach,” Cognition, vol. 69, no. 1, pp. 1–34, 1998.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413