全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Blind Sailors’ Spatial Representation Using an On-Board Force Feedback Arm: Two Case Studies

DOI: 10.1155/2013/163718

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Using a vocal, auditory, and haptic application designed for maritime navigation, blind sailors are able to set up and manage their voyages. However, investigation of the manner to present information remains a crucial issue to better understand spatial cognition and improve navigation without vision. In this study, we asked two participants to use SeaTouch on board and manage the ship headings during navigation in order to follow a predefined itinerary. Two conditions were tested. Firstly, blind sailors consulted the updated ship positions about the virtual map presented in an allocentric frame of reference (i.e., facing north). In the second case, they used the forced-feedback device in an egocentric frame of reference (i.e., facing the ship headings). Spatial performance tended to show that the egocentric condition was better for controlling the course during displacement, whereas the allocentric condition was more efficient for building mental representation and remembering it after the navigation task. 1. Introduction Nowadays, it is well known that blind people can take advantage of virtual navigation. Indeed, various experiments have shown interesting results in different types of environments. Jansson and Pedersen [1] studied a virtual map of North America’s States and showed that it was difficult to navigate with a haptic mouse (VTPlayer) which only provided participants with cutaneous feedback (two matrices of pins) in spite of the participants’ motivation. Gutierrez [2] used a forced-feedback device (GRAB Project) and assessed a geographical haptic representation of Madrid (Spain) by asking twelve blind people to create and depict a route between two points. Participants completed the tasks without major difficulty and stated that the application was attractive and easy to use because of the combination of forced-feedback and audio information. At the same time, Magnusson and Rassmus-Gr?hn [3] investigated the transfer between an egocentric haptic virtual environment and a real street in a district of Lund (Sweden). Here, they asked participants to prepare and realize an itinerary from a bus stop to a music hall. Results show that blind people who were good at navigating with a cane were also good at exploring with the haptic device. Then, in the street, participants were able to complete the itinerary learnt in the virtual environment. Therefore, a transfer happened between the virtual and real worlds. In this respect, Lahav and Mioduser [4] compared nonvisual spatial representations obtained after having explored real and virtual

References

[1]  G. Jansson and P. Pedersen, “Obtaining geographical information from a virtual map with a haptic mouse,” in Proceedings of the 22nd International Cartographic Conference (ICC '05), A Coruna, Spain, July 2005.
[2]  T. Gutierrez, Enhanced Network Accessibility for the Blind and Visually Impaired, Labein, Madrid, Spain, 2004.
[3]  C. Magnusson and K. Rassmus-Gr?hn, “A dynamic haptic-audio traffic environment,” in Proceedings of the 2004 EuroHaptics, Munich, Germany, June 2004.
[4]  O. Lahav and D. Mioduser, “Haptic-feedback support for cognitive mapping of unknown spaces by people who are blind,” International Journal of Human Computer Studies, vol. 66, no. 1, pp. 23–35, 2008.
[5]  M. Simonnet, S. Vieilledent, J. Guinard, and J. Tisseau, “Can haptic maps contribute to spatial knowledge of blind sailors?” in Proceedings of the International Conference on Enactive Interfaces (ENACTIVE '07), pp. 259–262, Grenoble, France, 2007.
[6]  M. Simonnet, S. Vieilledent, D. R. Jacobson, and J. Tisseau, “The assessment of non visual maritime cognitive maps of a blind sailor: a case study,” Journal of Maps, vol. 2010, pp. 289–301, 2010.
[7]  R. F. Wang and E. S. Spelke, “Human spatial representation: insights from animals,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 6, no. 9, pp. 376–382, 2002.
[8]  B. G. Witmer, J. H. Bailey, B. W. Knerr, and K. C. Parsons, “Virtual spaces and real world places: transfer of route knowledge,” International Journal of Human Computer Studies, vol. 45, no. 4, pp. 413–428, 1996.
[9]  W. Rodes and L. Gugerty, “Effects of electronic map displays and individual differences in ability on navigation performance,” Human Factors, vol. 54, no. 4, pp. 589–599, 2012.
[10]  A. Friedman and B. Kohler, “Bidimensional regression: assessing the configural similarity and accuracy of cognitive maps and other two-dimensional data sets,” Psychological Methods, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 468–491, 2003.
[11]  C. Thinus-Blanc and F. Gaunet, “Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense?” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 121, no. 1, pp. 20–42, 1997.
[12]  W. Mou, H. Zhang, and T. P. McNamara, “Novel-view scene recognition relies on identifying spatial reference directions,” Cognition, vol. 111, no. 2, pp. 175–186, 2009.
[13]  W. Mou, Y. Fan, T. P. McNamara, and C. B. Owen, “Intrinsic frames of reference and egocentric viewpoints in scene recognition,” Cognition, vol. 106, no. 2, pp. 750–769, 2008.
[14]  C. D. Wickens, “Human factors in vector map design: the importance of task-display dependence,” Journal of Navigation, vol. 53, no. 1, pp. 54–67, 2000.
[15]  T. Porathe, “Measuring effective map design for route guidance: an experiment comparing electronic map display principles,” Information Design Journal, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 190–201, 2008.
[16]  J. F. Fletcher, “Spatial representation in blind children. 1: development compared to sighted children,” Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, vol. 74, no. 10, pp. 381–385, 1980.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413