全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Presence of Contagious Yawning in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

DOI: 10.1155/2013/971686

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Most previous studies suggest diminished susceptibility to contagious yawning in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, it could be driven by their atypical attention to the face. To test this hypothesis, children with ASD and typically developing (TD) children were shown yawning and control movies. To ensure participants' attention to the face, an eye tracker controlled the onset of the yawning and control stimuli. Results demonstrated that both TD children and children with ASD yawned more frequently when they watched the yawning stimuli than the control stimuli. It is suggested therefore that the absence of contagious yawning in children with ASD, as reported in previous studies, might relate to their weaker tendency to spontaneously attend to others' faces. 1. Introduction Yawning is widespread among vertebrate species, including a wide range of mammals [1]. In humans, yawning is detectable even in the foetus [2]. The function of yawning is still unclear, but a recent theory highlighted that it may have a communicative function [3]. This hypothesis suggests that yawning is a nonverbal form of communication that synchronizes the behavior of a group [4–7]. It has been suggested that yawning transmits physiological and psychological states, such as drowsiness [8, 9], boredom [10], hunger, and mild psychological stress [7], to other members of the group. Among the evidence that supports this theory, studies of contagious yawning have attracted the most attention in recent years. Contagious yawning, in which observation of another’s yawn induces yawning behaviour in the observer, is a well-documented phenomenon. In humans, contagious yawning can be elicited by viewing or hearing others’ yawning or imagining yawning (e.g., [5, 11–14]). During the course of development, contagious yawning can be reliably observed by around 4 to 6 years of age [15, 16] but might not be present in younger infants and toddlers [17]. The presence of contagious yawning has also been reported in several nonhuman animals (e.g., [18–20]). Several neuroimaging studies have been conducted to investigate the cortical and subcortical structures relevant to the contagious yawning [21–24], but the results are inconsistent. This is possibly due to differences in the yawning stimuli and/or control stimuli used for recording [25]. To date, three independent studies have consistently demonstrated the absence of contagious yawning in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a developmental disorder with profound impairments in social interaction and communication

References

[1]  R. Baenninger, “Some comparative aspects of yawning in Betta splendens, Homo sapiens, Panthera leo, and Papio sphinx,” Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol. 101, no. 4, pp. 349–354, 1987.
[2]  W. Sepulveda and M. Mangiamarchi, “Fetal yawning,” Ultrasound in Obstetrics & Gynecology, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 57–59, 1995.
[3]  A. G. Guggisberg, J. Mathis, A. Schnider, and C. W. Hess, “Why do we yawn?” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 34, no. 8, pp. 1267–1276, 2010.
[4]  J. Barbizet, “Yawning,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 203–209, 1958.
[5]  R. R. Provine, “Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releasing stimulus,” Ethology, vol. 72, no. 2, pp. 109–122, 1986.
[6]  M. P. I. Weller, “Yawning,” The Lancet, vol. 1, no. 8591, p. 950, 1988.
[7]  B. L. Deputte, “Ethological study of yawning in primates. 1.Quantitative analysis and study of causation in two species of Old World monkeys (Cercocebus albigena and Macaca fascicularis),” Ethology, vol. 98, no. 3-4, pp. 221–245, 1994.
[8]  I. Zilli, F. Giganti, and V. Uga, “Yawning and subjective sleepiness in the elderly,” Journal of Sleep Research, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 303–308, 2008.
[9]  A. G. Guggisberg, J. Mathis, U. S. Herrmann, and C. W. Hess, “The functional relationship between yawning and vigilance,” Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 179, no. 1, pp. 159–166, 2007.
[10]  R. R. Provine and H. B. Hamernik, “Yawning-effects of stimulus interest,” Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, vol. 24, no. 6, pp. 437–438, 1986.
[11]  R. Baenninger and M. Greco, “Some antecedents and consequences of yawning,” Psychological Record, vol. 41, no. 4, pp. 453–460, 1991.
[12]  F. Giganti and M. Esposito Ziello, “Contagious and spontaneous yawning in autistic and typically developing children,” Current Psychology Letters, vol. 25, no. 1, 2009.
[13]  H. E. Lehmann, “Yawning. A homeostatic reflex and its psychological significance,” Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, vol. 43, no. 2, pp. 123–136, 1979.
[14]  R. R. Provine, “Faces as releasers of contagious yawning-an approach to face detection using normal human-subjects,” Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 211–214, 1989.
[15]  J. R. Anderson and P. Meno, “Psychological influences on yawning in children,” Current Psychology Letters, vol. 11, no. 2, 2003.
[16]  M. S. Helt, I. Eigsti, P. J. Snyder, and D. A. Fein, “Contagious yawning in autistic and typical development,” Child Development, vol. 81, no. 5, pp. 1620–1631, 2010.
[17]  A. Millen and J. R. Anderson, “Neither infants nor toddlers catch yawns from their mothers,” Biology Letters, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 440–442, 2011.
[18]  J. R. Anderson, M. Myowa-Yamakoshi, and T. Matsuzawa, “Contagious yawning in chimpanzees,” Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol. 271, supplement 6, pp. S468–S470, 2004.
[19]  R. M. Joly-Mascheroni, A. Senju, and A. J. Shepherd, “Dogs catch human yawns,” Biology Letters, vol. 4, no. 5, pp. 446–448, 2008.
[20]  E. Palagi, A. Leone, G. Mancini, and P. F. Ferrari, “Contagious yawning in gelada baboons as a possible expression of empathy,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 106, no. 46, pp. 19262–19267, 2009.
[21]  S. R. Arnott, A. Singhal, and M. A. Goodale, “An investigation of auditory contagious yawning,” Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 335–342, 2009.
[22]  F. B. Nahab, N. Hattori, Z. S. Saad, and M. Hallett, “Contagious yawning and the frontal lobe: an fMRI study,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 1744–1751, 2009.
[23]  S. M. Platek, F. B. Mohamed, and G. G. Gallup Jr., “Contagious yawning and the brain,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 23, no. 2-3, pp. 448–452, 2005.
[24]  M. Schürmann, M. D. Hesse, K. E. Stephan et al., “Yearning to yawn: the neural basis of contagious yawning,” NeuroImage, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 1260–1264, 2005.
[25]  F. B. Nahab, “Exploring yawning with neuroimaging,” Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience, vol. 28, pp. 128–133, 2010.
[26]  American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA, 4th edition, 2000.
[27]  A. Senju, M. Maeda, Y. Kikuchi, T. Hasegawa, Y. Tojo, and H. Osanai, “Absence of contagious yawning in children with autism spectrum disorder,” Biology Letters, vol. 3, no. 6, pp. 706–708, 2007.
[28]  K. Chawarska, F. Volkmar, and A. Klin, “Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 67, no. 2, pp. 178–185, 2010.
[29]  Y. Kikuchi, A. Senju, H. Akechi, Y. Tojo, H. Osanai, and T. Hasegawa, “Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 41, no. 5, pp. 629–645, 2011.
[30]  S. Maestro, F. Muratori, M. C. Cavallaro et al., “How young children treat objects and people: an empirical study of the first year of life in autism,” Child Psychiatry and Human Development, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 383–396, 2005.
[31]  A. Senju, Y. Kikuchi, H. Akechi, T. Hasegawa, Y. Tojo, and H. Osanai, “Brief report: does eye contact induce contagious yawning in children with autism spectrum disorder?” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 39, no. 11, pp. 1598–1602, 2009.
[32]  H. Akechi, A. Senju, Y. Kikuchi, Y. Tojo, H. Osanai, and T. Hasegawa, “Do children with ASD use referential gaze to learn the name of an object? An eye-tracking study,” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 1230–1242, 2011.
[33]  A. Senju and G. Csibra, “Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals,” Current Biology, vol. 18, no. 9, pp. 668–671, 2008.
[34]  A. Senju, V. Southgate, S. White, and U. Frith, “Mindblind eyes: an absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome,” Science, vol. 325, no. 5942, pp. 883–885, 2009.
[35]  S. K. Berument, M. Rutter, C. Lord, A. Pickles, and A. Bailey, “Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic validity,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 175, pp. 444–451, 1999.
[36]  H. Dairoku, A. Senju, E. Hayashi, Y. Tojo, and H. Ichikawa, “Development of Japanese version of autism screening questionnaire,” Kokuritsu Tokushu Kyoiku Sougou Kenkyusho Bunshitsu Ippan Kenkyu Houkokusho, vol. B-184, pp. 19–34, 2004.
[37]  D. Wechsler, The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Psychological Corporation, London, UK, 3rd edition, 1992.
[38]  Japanese WISC-III Publication Committee, Nihonban WISC-III Chinou Kensahoued, Japanese Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Nihon Bunka Kagakusha, Tokyo, Japan, 3rd edition, 1998.
[39]  D. Wechsler, “The psychometric tradition: developing the wechsler adult intelligence scale,” Contemporary Educational Psychology, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 82–85, 1981.
[40]  F. Shinagawa, S. Kobayashi, K. Fujita, and H. Maekawa, Nihonban WAIS-R Seijin Chinou Kensahou, A Japanese version of the WAIS-R, Nihon Bunka Kagakusha, Tokyo, Japan, 1990.
[41]  L. M. Oberman and V. S. Ramachandran, “The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 133, no. 2, pp. 310–327, 2007.
[42]  M. J. C. M. Magnée, B. De Gelder, H. Van Engeland, and C. Kemner, “Facial electromyographic responses to emotional information from faces and voices in individuals with pervasive developmental disorder,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 48, no. 11, pp. 1122–1130, 2007.
[43]  N. Hadjikhani, R. M. Joseph, J. Snyder et al., “Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces,” NeuroImage, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 1141–1150, 2004.
[44]  N. Hadjikhani, R. H. Joseph, J. Snyder, and H. Tager-Flusberg, “Abnormal activation of the social brain during face perception in autism,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 28, no. 5, pp. 441–449, 2007.
[45]  S. B. Perlman, C. M. Hudac, T. Pegors, N. J. Minshew, and K. A. Pelphrey, “Experimental manipulation of face-evoked activity in the fusiform gyrus of individuals with autism,” Social Neuroscience, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 22–30, 2011.
[46]  H. Haker and W. R?ssler, “Empathy in schizophrenia: impaired resonance,” European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 259, no. 6, pp. 352–361, 2009.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133