全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Verbal Comprehension Ability in Aphasia: Demographic and Lexical Knowledge Effects

DOI: 10.1155/2014/258303

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Background. Assessment of sentence-level auditory comprehension can be performed with a variety of tests varying in response requirements. A brief and easy to administer measure, not requiring an overt verbal or a complex motor response, is essential in any test battery for aphasia. Objective. The present study examines the clinical utility of receptive language indices for individuals with aphasia based on the Comprehension of Instructions in Greek (CIG), a variant of the Token Test, and the Greek version of PPVT-R. Methods. Normative data from a large community sample of Greek adults aged 46–80 years was available on both measures. A word-level-independent measure of auditory comprehension was computed as the standard score difference between the two tests and used to compare patients with and without comprehension deficits as indicated by their Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination profile. Results and Conclusions. Indices of internal consistency and test-retest reliability were very good. Education and age effects on performance were significant, with the former being stronger. The potential clinical utility of differential ability indices (contrasting sentence- and word-level auditory comprehension tests) is discussed. 1. Introduction Auditory comprehension is one of the major components of general linguistic ability and many individuals with aphasia demonstrate comprehension deficits. These deficits are commonly associated with lesions in various left hemisphere regions, including the posterior middle temporal gyrus, the anterior superior temporal gyrus, the superior temporal sulcus, the angular gyrus, and frontal areas BA 46 and BA 47 [1]. In general, auditory comprehension is assessed at two levels: at the word level (word level auditory comprehension, henceforth WLAC) and at the sentence level (sentence level auditory comprehension, henceforth SLAC). In clinical settings, comprehension of spoken sentences is considered more critical and predictive of overall linguistic and social functioning and requires a set of intact cognitive functions, including lexical/semantic access (primarily tapped by WLAC tasks), syntactic processing, and working memory [2–4]. The ability to extract meaning from spoken sentences can be assessed with a variety of task formats. Some tests were specifically designed to assess processing of particular syntactic sentence frames, such as the Sentence Comprehension Test of the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences [5, 6], the Subject-relative, Object-relative, Active, Passive syntactic battery [7], and the

References

[1]  N. F. Dronkers, D. P. Wilkins, R. D. Van Valin Jr., B. B. Redfern, and J. J. Jaeger, “Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension,” Cognition, vol. 92, no. 1-2, pp. 145–177, 2004.
[2]  M. D. Lezak, D. B. Howieson, and D. W. Loring, Neuropsychological Assessment, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2004.
[3]  J. D. Noll and S. R. Randolph, “Auditory semantic, syntactic, and retention errors made by aphasic subjects on the token test,” Journal of Communication Disorders, vol. 11, no. 6, pp. 543–553, 1978.
[4]  R. Lesser, “Verbal and non verbal memory components in the token test,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 79–85, 1976.
[5]  S. Cho-Reyes and C. K. Thompson, “Verb and sentence production and comprehension in aphasia: Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS),” Aphasiology, vol. 26, pp. 1250–1277, 2012.
[6]  C. K. Thompson, A. Meltzer-Asscher, S. Cho et al., “Syntactic and morphosyntactic processing in stroke-induced and primary progressive aphasia,” Behavioural Neurology, vol. 26, pp. 35–54, 2013.
[7]  T. Love and E. Oster, “On the categorization of aphasie typologies: the SOAP (a test of syntactic complexity),” Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, vol. 31, no. 5, pp. 503–529, 2002.
[8]  M. Paradis, “Principles underlying the bilingual aphasia test (BAT) and its uses,” Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, vol. 25, no. 6-7, pp. 427–443, 2011.
[9]  M. Kambanaros and K. K. Grohmann, “BATting multilingual primary progressive aphasia for Greek, English, and Czech,” Journal of Neurolinguistics, vol. 25, pp. 520–537, 2012.
[10]  K. Tsapkini, G. Jarema, and E. Kehayia, “A morphological processing deficit in verbs but not in nouns: a case study in a highly inflected language,” Journal of Neurolinguistics, vol. 15, no. 3–5, pp. 265–288, 2002.
[11]  H. Goodglass and E. Kaplan, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia, Pa, USA, 1983.
[12]  K. Tsapkini, C. H. Vlahou, and C. Potagas, “Adaptation and validation of standardized aphasia tests in different languages: lessons from the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination—short form in Greek,” Behavioural Neurology, vol. 22, no. 3-4, pp. 111–119, 2009.
[13]  A. Kertesz, Western Aphasia Battery, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 1982.
[14]  E. de Renzi and L. A. Vignolo, “The token test: a sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics,” Brain, vol. 85, no. 4, pp. 665–678, 1962.
[15]  O. Spreen and E. Strauss, A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: administration, Norms, and Commentary, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1998.
[16]  J. G. Beaumont, J. Marjoribanks, S. Flury, and T. Lintern, “Assessing auditory comprehension in the context of severe physical disability: the PACST,” Brain Injury, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 99–112, 1999.
[17]  R. J. Ivnik, “Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE Token, WRAT-R Reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO,” Clinical Neuropsychologist, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 262–278, 1996.
[18]  B. Orgass and K. Poeck, “Clinical validation of a new test for aphasia: an experimental study on the Token Test,” Cortex, vol. 2, pp. 222–243, 1966.
[19]  L. L. Mansur, M. Radanovic, L. Taquemori, L. Greco, and G. C. Araújo, “A study of the abilities in oral language comprehension of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination—Portuguese version: a reference guide for the Brazilian population,” Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, vol. 38, no. 2, pp. 277–292, 2005.
[20]  P. G. Simos, D. Kasselimis, and A. Mouzaki, “Age, gender, and education effects on vocabulary measures in Greek,” Aphasiology, vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 475–491, 2011.
[21]  H. Gardner, M. L. Albert, and S. Weintraub, “Comprehending a word: the influence of speed and redundancy on auditory comprehension in aphasia,” Cortex, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 155–162, 1975.
[22]  K. M. Heilman and R. J. Scholes, “The nature of comprehension errors in Broca's conduction and Wernicke's aphasics,” Cortex, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 258–265, 1976.
[23]  D. B. Hier, S. I. Mogil, N. P. Rubin, and G. R. Komros, “Semantic aphasia: a neglected entity,” Brain and Language, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 120–131, 1980.
[24]  G. L. Wallace and J. H. Stapleton, “Analysis of auditory comprehension performance in individuals with severe aphasia,” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 72, no. 9, pp. 674–678, 1991.
[25]  L. M. Dunn and E. S. Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minn, USA, 1981.
[26]  J. P. Culbert, R. Hamer, and V. Klinge, “Factor structure of the Wechsler Intelligence scale for Children-Revised, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, and the Peabody Individual Assessment Test in a psychiatric sample,” Psychology in the Schools, vol. 26, pp. 331–336, 1989.
[27]  R. W. Payne and G. Jones, “Statistics for the investigation of individual cases,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 115–121, 1957.
[28]  J. C. Arvedson, M. R. McNeil, and T. L. West, “Prediction of revised Token Test overall, subtest, and linguistic unit scores by two shortened versions,” in Clinical Aphasiology Conference, pp. 57–63, BRK Publishers, Rockville, Md, USA, 1985.
[29]  E. de Renzi and P. Faglioni, “Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the token test,” Cortex, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 41–49, 1978.
[30]  B. Hallowell, R. T. Wertz, and H. Kruse, “Using eye movement responses to index auditory comprehension: an adaptation of the Revised Token Test,” Aphasiology, vol. 16, no. 4–6, pp. 587–594, 2002.
[31]  M. M. McNeil and T. E. Prescott, Revised Token Test, Pro-Ed, Austin, Tex, USA, 1978.
[32]  B. Orgass, “Eine Revision des Token Tests II. Validit?tsnachweis, Normierung und Standardisierung,” Diagnostica, vol. 22, pp. 141–156, 1976.
[33]  O. Spreen and A. L. Benton, The Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia. Neuropsychology Laboratory, University of Victoria, 1969, 1977.
[34]  A. L. Benton, Hamsher, K. de S, and A. B. Sivan, Multilingual Aphasia Examination, AJA Associates, Iowa City, Iowa, USA, 3rd edition, 1994.
[35]  M. Korkman, U. Kirk, and S. Kemp, NEPSY, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 1998.
[36]  G. Blom, Statistical Estimates and Transformed Beta Variables, John Wiley and Sons, New York, NY, USA, 1958.
[37]  J. A. Lucas, R. J. Ivnik, G. E. Smith et al., “Mayo's older African Americans normative studies: Norms for Boston naming test, controlled oral word association, category fluency, animal naming, token test, WRAT-3 reading, trail making test, stroop test, and judgment of line orientation,” Clinical Neuropsychologist, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 243–269, 2005.
[38]  J. B. Rich, Pictorial and verbal implicit and recognition memory in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: a transfer-appropriate processing account [Doctoral dissertation], University of Victoria, 1993.
[39]  O. B. Emery, “Linguistic decrement in normal aging,” Language and Communication, vol. 6, no. 1-2, pp. 47–64, 1986.
[40]  E. Strauss, E. M. S. Sherman, and O. Spreen, A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 3rd edition, 2006.
[41]  H. Braak and E. Braak, “Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 351–357, 1997.
[42]  R. L. Buckner, “Memory and executive function in aging and ad: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate,” Neuron, vol. 44, no. 1, pp. 195–208, 2004.
[43]  F. A. Schmitt, D. G. Davis, D. R. Wekstein, C. D. Smith, J. W. Ashford, and W. R. Markesbery, “‘Preclinical’ AD revisited: neuropathology of cognitively normal older adults,” Neurology, vol. 55, no. 3, pp. 370–376, 2000.
[44]  P. G. Simos, D. Kasselimis, and A. Mouzaki, “Effects of demographic variables and health status on brief vocabulary measures in Greek,” Aphasiology, vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 492–504, 2011.
[45]  K. W. Schaie, “The development of intelligence in adulthood,” Zeitschrift für Gerontologie, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 373–384, 1980.
[46]  C. Schooler, “Psychosocial factors and effective cognitive functioning in adulthood,” in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie, Eds., pp. 347–358, Academic Press, San Diego, Calif, USA, 3rd edition, 1990.
[47]  R. F. Zec, “The neuropsychology of aging,” Experimental Gerontology, vol. 30, no. 3-4, pp. 431–442, 1995.
[48]  A. Ardila, F. Ostrosky-Solis, M. Rosselli, and C. Gómez, “Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 15, no. 6, pp. 495–513, 2000.
[49]  A. Patricacou, E. Psallida, T. Pring, and L. Dipper, “The Boston naming test in Greek: normative data and the effects of age and education on naming,” Aphasiology, vol. 21, no. 12, pp. 1157–1170, 2007.
[50]  F. Ostrosky-Solis, A. Ardila, M. Rosselli, G. Lopez-Arango, and V. Uriel-Mendoza, “Neuropsychological test performance in illiterate subjects,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 13, no. 7, pp. 645–660, 1998.
[51]  B. A. Steinberg, L. A. Bieliauskas, G. E. Smith, C. Langellotti, and R. J. Ivnik, “Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Boston Naming Test, the MAE Token Test, and the Judgment of Line Orientation Test,” Clinical Neuropsychologist, vol. 19, no. 3-4, pp. 280–328, 2005.
[52]  D. V. Cicchetti, “Multiple comparison methods: establishing guidelines for their valid application in neuropsychological research,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 155–161, 1994.
[53]  S. Gao and K. Sun, “The special-category semantic disturbance of auditory comprehension of aphasics,” Chinese Journal of Neurology, vol. 35, no. 6, pp. 330–332, 2002.
[54]  C. Humphries, J. R. Binder, D. A. Medler, and E. Liebenthal, “Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 665–679, 2006.
[55]  K. Jodzio, D. Biechowska, and B. Leszniewska-Jodzio, “Selectivity of lexical-semantic disorders in Polish-speaking patients with aphasia: evidence from single-word comprehension,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 23, no. 5, pp. 543–551, 2008.
[56]  E. Peristeri and K. Tsapkini, “A comparison of the BAT and BDAE-SF batteries in determining the linguistic ability in Greek-speaking patients with Broca's aphasia,” Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, vol. 25, no. 6-7, pp. 464–479, 2011.
[57]  D. Caplan, “Aphasic deficits in syntactic processing,” Cortex, vol. 42, no. 6, pp. 797–804, 2006.
[58]  J. R. Crawford, D. C. Howell, and P. H. Garthwaite, “Payne and Jones revisited: estimating the abnormality of test score differences using a modified paired samples t test,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 20, no. 6, pp. 898–905, 1998.
[59]  A. M. Poreh, The Quantified Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment, Taylor and Francis, New York, NY, USA, 2006.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413