全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Cognitive Flexibility, Theory of Mind, and Hyperactivity/Inattention

DOI: 10.1155/2014/741543

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

The present study analyzed the concurrent and longitudinal relations among cognitive flexibility, theory of mind, and hyperactivity/inattention in a sample of 70 typically developing children ( age = 61.4 months, SD = 8.3 months). Mothers and teachers reported on children’s hyperactivity/inattention using the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (Goodman, 1997), cognitive flexibility was measured using the dimension change card sort task (Zelazo, 2006), and theory of mind was assessed using a battery of tasks. Cognitive flexibility and theory of mind scores were found to be significantly negatively correlated with the level of hyperactivity/inattention at both time points. Furthermore, year 1 cognitive flexibility score was found to be a significant predictor of year 2 hyperactivity/inattention score after controlling for child age, gender, and year 1 hyperactivity/inattention score. Directions for future research include training studies which would further our understanding of these relationships and allow more effective interventions. 1. Introduction The ability to focus attention and regulate behavior is a key determinant of scholastic achievement and occupational success [1–3]. Indeed, these self-control skills are viewed as desirable by parents, teachers, and employers alike [3] and high levels of problems with focusing or sustaining attention and regulating behavior are core clinical features used to diagnose attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, many children with symptoms of hyperactivity and inattention do not meet the criteria for a formal ADHD diagnosis and parents and education professionals are confronted with the challenge of working with these children [4]. The present study investigated the cognitive factors associated with symptoms of hyperactivity/inattention in typically developing children during the early school years. Much of the research into the development of children’s self-regulation skills has been conducted within the executive function framework [5]. Inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility are components of executive function that contribute to the development of self-regulation [6]. These executive function components follow different trajectories across early development such that the preschool period is characterized by dramatic improvements in inhibitory control, whereas the development of cognitive flexibility accelerates during the early school years [7]. According to cognitive complexity and control (CCC) theory, cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to flexibly

References

[1]  S. Normandeau and F. Guay, “Preschool behavior and first-grade school achievement: the mediational role of cognitive self-control,” Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 90, no. 1, pp. 111–121, 1998.
[2]  L. S. Fuchs, D. Fuchs, D. L. Compton et al., “The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems,” Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 98, no. 1, pp. 29–43, 2006.
[3]  G. Farkas, “Cognitive skills and noncognitive traits and behaviors in stratification processes,” Annual Review of Sociology, vol. 29, pp. 541–562, 2003.
[4]  M. K. Demaray and L. N. Jenkins, “Relations among academic enablers and academic achievement in children with and without high levels of parent-rated symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity,” Psychology in the Schools, vol. 48, no. 6, pp. 573–586, 2011.
[5]  Q. Zhou, S. H. Chen, and A. Main, “Commonalities and differences in the research on children's effortful control and executive function: a call for an integrated model of self-regulation,” Child Development Perspectives, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 112–121, 2012.
[6]  M. M. McClelland and C. E. Cameron, “Self-regulation in early childhood: improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures,” Child Development Perspectives, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 136–142, 2012.
[7]  C. Hughes, “Changes and challenges in 20 years of research into the development of executive functions,” Infant and Child Development, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 251–271, 2011.
[8]  S. Jacques and P. D. Zelazo, “On the possible roots of cognitive flexibility,” in The Development of Social Cognition and Communication, pp. 53–81, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005.
[9]  S. Jacques and P. D. Zelazo, “Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: implications for theory of mind,” in Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind, J. W. Astington and J. A. Baird, Eds., pp. 144–162, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.
[10]  P. D. Zelazo, U. Müller, D. Frye et al., “The development of executive function: cognitive complexity and control—revised,” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 93–119, 2003.
[11]  K. Happaney and P. D. Zelazo, “Inhibition as a problem in the psychology of behavior,” Developmental Science, vol. 6, no. 5, pp. 468–470, 2003.
[12]  G. M. Marzocchi, J. Oosterlaan, A. Zuddas et al., “Contrasting deficits on executive functions between ADHD and reading disabled children,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 49, no. 5, pp. 543–552, 2008.
[13]  M. J. Reader, E. L. Harris, L. J. Schuerholz, and M. B. Denckla, “Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and executive dysfunction,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 493–512, 1994.
[14]  J. Holmes, S. E. Gathercole, M. Place, T. P. Alloway, J. G. Elliott, and K. A. Hilton, “The diagnostic utility of executive function assessments in the identification of ADHD in Children,” Child and Adolescent Mental Health, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 37–43, 2010.
[15]  M. Martel, M. Nikolas, and J. T. Nigg, “Executive function in adolescents with ADHD,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 46, no. 11, pp. 1437–1444, 2007.
[16]  H. M. Geurts, S. Verté, J. Oosterlaan, H. Roeyers, and J. A. Sergeant, “How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and autism?” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 4, pp. 836–854, 2004.
[17]  B. A. Corbett, L. J. Constantine, R. Hendren, D. Rocke, and S. Ozonoff, “Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 166, no. 2-3, pp. 210–222, 2009.
[18]  E. G. Willcutt, A. E. Doyle, J. T. Nigg, S. V. Faraone, and B. F. Pennington, “Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 57, no. 11, pp. 1336–1346, 2005.
[19]  D. Premack and G. Woodruff, “Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 1, no. 4, pp. 515–526, 1978.
[20]  D. Frye, P. D. Zelazo, and J. A. Burack, “Cognitive complexity and control. I: theory of mind in typical and atypical development,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 7, no. 4, pp. 116–121, 1998.
[21]  P. D. Zelazo, “The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children,” Nature Protocols, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 297–301, 2006.
[22]  D. Frye, P. D. Zelazo, and T. Palfai, “Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning,” Cognitive Development, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 483–527, 1995.
[23]  B. Lang and J. Perner, “Understanding of intention and false belief and the development of self-control,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 67–76, 2002.
[24]  U. Müller, P. D. Zelazo, and S. Imrisek, “Executive function and children's understanding of false belief: how specific is the relation?” Cognitive Development, vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 173–189, 2005.
[25]  J. Perner, B. Lang, and D. Kloo, “Theory of mind and self-control: more than a common problem of inhibition,” Child Development, vol. 73, no. 3, pp. 752–767, 2002.
[26]  G. Andrews, G. S. Halford, K. M. Bunch, D. Bowden, and T. Jones, “Theory of mind and relational complexity,” Child Development, vol. 74, no. 5, pp. 1476–1499, 2003.
[27]  E. Colvert, D. Custance, and J. Swettenham, “Rule-based reasoning and theory of mind in autism: a commentary on the work of Zelazo, Jacques, Burack and Frye,” Infant and Child Development, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 197–200, 2002.
[28]  B. M. Farrant, M. T. Maybery, and J. Fletcher, “Language, cognitive flexibility, and explicit false belief understanding: longitudinal lnalysis in typical development and specific language impairment,” Child Development, vol. 83, no. 1, pp. 223–235, 2012.
[29]  A. Nydén, L. Niklasson, O. Stahlberg et al., “Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological aspects,” Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 31, no. 6, pp. 1659–1668, 2010.
[30]  J. K. Buitelaar, M. van der Wees, H. Swaab-Barneveld, and R. J. van der Gaag, “Theory of mind and emotion-recognition functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control and normal children,” Development and Psychopathology, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 39–58, 1999.
[31]  J. K. Buitelaar, H. Swaab, M. van der Wees, M. Wildschut, and R. J. van der Gaag, “Neuropsychological impairments and deficits in theory of mind and emotion recognition in a non-autistic boy,” European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 44–51, 1996.
[32]  J. Perner, W. Kain, and P. Barchfeld, “Executive control and higher-order theory of mind in children at risk of ADHD,” Infant and Child Development, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 141–158, 2002.
[33]  T. Charman, F. Carroll, and C. Sturge, “Theory of mind, executive function and social competence in boys with ADHD,” Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 31–49, 2001.
[34]  M. J. Dyck, K. Ferguson, and I. M. Shochet, “Do autism spectrum disorders differ from each other and from non-spectrum disorders on emotion recognition tests?” European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 105–116, 2001.
[35]  T. C. Papadopoulos, G. Panayiotou, G. Spanoudis, and D. Natsopoulos, “Evidence of poor planning in children with attention deficits,” Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, vol. 33, no. 5, pp. 611–623, 2005.
[36]  C. M. Fahie and D. K. Symons, “Executive functioning and theory of mind in children clinically referred for attention and behavior problems,” Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, vol. 24, no. 1, pp. 51–73, 2003.
[37]  D. Hongwanishkul, K. R. Happaney, W. S. C. Lee, and P. D. Zelazo, “Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 617–644, 2005.
[38]  H. M. Wellman and D. Liu, “Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks,” Child Development, vol. 75, no. 2, pp. 523–541, 2004.
[39]  B. M. Farrant, J. Fletcher, and M. T. Maybery, “Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language,” Child Development, vol. 77, no. 6, pp. 1842–1853, 2006.
[40]  T. Woolfe, S. C. Want, and M. Siegal, “Signposts to development: theory of mind in deaf children,” Child Development, vol. 73, no. 3, pp. 768–778, 2002.
[41]  K. Sullivan and H. Tager-Flusberg, “Second-order belief attribution in Williams syndrome: intact or impaired?” American Journal on Mental Retardation, vol. 104, no. 6, pp. 523–532, 1999.
[42]  C. C. Peterson, H. M. Wellman, and D. Liu, “Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism,” Child Development, vol. 76, no. 2, pp. 502–517, 2005.
[43]  R. Goodman, “The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 38, no. 5, pp. 581–586, 1997.
[44]  A. Goodman and R. Goodman, “Population mean scores predict child mental disorder rates: validating SDQ prevalence estimators in Britain,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 52, no. 1, pp. 100–108, 2011.
[45]  R. Goodman, “Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 40, no. 11, pp. 1337–1345, 2001.
[46]  T. Lewis, J. Kotch, R. Thompson et al., “Witnessed violence and youth behavior problems: a multi-informant study,” American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, vol. 80, no. 4, pp. 443–450, 2010.
[47]  E. Youngstrom, R. Loeber, and M. Stouthamer-Loeber, “Patterns and correlates of agreement between parent, teacher, and male adolescent ratings of externalizing and internalizing problems,” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, vol. 68, no. 6, pp. 1038–1050, 2000.
[48]  J. M. Nelson and H. R. Harwood, “A meta-analysis of parent and teacher reports of depression among students with learning disabilities: evidence for the importance of multi-informant assessment,” Psychology in the Schools, vol. 48, no. 4, pp. 371–384, 2011.
[49]  K. J. Preacher and A. F. Hayes, “Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models,” Behavior Research Methods, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 879–891, 2008.
[50]  C. Hughes and R. Ensor, “Does executive function matter for preschoolers' problem behaviors?” Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, vol. 36, no. 1, pp. 1–14, 2008.
[51]  N. Garon, S. E. Bryson, and I. M. Smith, “Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 31–60, 2008.
[52]  S. M. Dowsett and D. J. Livesey, “The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: effects of “executive skills” training,” Developmental Psychobiology, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 161–174, 2000.
[53]  L. Bradley and P. Bryant, “Categorizing sounds and learning to read: a causal connection,” Nature, vol. 301, no. 5899, pp. 419–421, 1983.
[54]  P. L. Harris, “Conversation, pretense, and theory of mind,” in Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind, J. W. Astington and J. A. Baird, Eds., pp. 70–83, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.
[55]  A. Mazaheri, C. Fassbender, S. Coffey-Corina, et al., “Differential oscillatory electroencephalogram between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes and typically developing adolescents,” Biological Psychiatry, 2013.
[56]  J. Pingault, S. Cote, C. Galera, et al., “Childhood trajectories of inattention, hyperactivity and oppositional behaviors and prediction of substance abuse/dependence: a 15-year longitudinal population-based study,” Molecular Psychiatry, vol. 18, no. 7, pp. 806–812, 2013.
[57]  A. K. Ulleb?, M.-B. Posserud, E. Heiervang, C. Gillberg, and C. Obel, “Screening for the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder phenotype using the strength and difficulties questionnaire,” European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 20, no. 9, pp. 451–458, 2011.
[58]  C. K. Conners, Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scale Manual, Multi-Health Systems, Ontario, Canada, 2008.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413