全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Object Individuation or Object Movement as Attractor? A Replication of the Wide-Screen/Narrow-Screen Study by Means of (a) Standard Looking Time Methodology and (b) Eye Tracking

DOI: 10.1155/2013/639702

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

We report a replication experiment of a mechanized version of the seminal wide-screen/narrow-screen design of Wilcox and Baillargeon (1998) with 9.5-month-old infants ( ). Two different methodologies were employed simultaneously: (a) the standard looking time paradigm and (b) eye tracking. Across conditions with three different screen sizes, the results from both methodologies revealed a clear and interesting pattern: the looking times increased as a significantly linear function of reduced screen sizes, that is, independently of the number of different objects involved. There was no indication in the data that the infants made use of the featural differences between the different-looking objects involved. The results suggest a simple, novel, and thought-provoking interpretation of the infants’ looking behavior in the wide-screen/narrow-screen design: moving objects are attractors, and the more space left for visible object movement in the visual field, the longer are infants’ looks. Consequently, no cognitive interpretation may be needed. 1. Introduction Object individuation refers to the process of deciding the number of distinct objects present in a given scenario (for reviews, see [1, 2]). In a now seminal paper, Xu and Carey [3] provided evidence that 10-month-old infants could only successfully individuate objects if they were given unequivocal spatiotemporal information about the number of objects present, whereas featural information was insufficient. Shortly after, Wilcox and Baillargeon [4] argued that the results obtained by Xu and Carey [3] could have been a consequence of the cognitively demanding design employed, rather than a general inability to use featural information when attempting to individuate objects. In order to substantiate their claim, Wilcox and Baillargeon [4] devised a simpler version of the design originally used by Xu and Carey—the so-called wide-screen/narrow-screen design. In the wide-screen/narrow-screen design, infants are presented with a basic design in which an object (e.g., a green ball) moves behind a screen. At the moment when one would expect the ball to reappear on the other side of the screen, a different-looking object (e.g., a red box) appears. The box stops at an outer location, rests for a brief period, and then returns behind the screen from which the ball reappears on the other side. This sequence continues until trial termination. Two different conditions are involved: a wide-screen condition, where the screen is of sufficient width to hide both objects at the same time (the expected condition) and a

References

[1]  P. Kr?jgaard, “A review of object individuation in infancy,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 159–183, 2004.
[2]  P. Kr?jgaard, “The human ability to single out and track specific objects trough space and time: origin and application,” in Human Characteristics: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Mind and Kind, H. H?gh-Olesen, P. Bertelsen, and J. T?nnesvang, Eds., pp. 89–116, Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 2009.
[3]  F. Xu and S. Carey, “Infants' metaphysics: the case of numerical identity,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 111–153, 1996.
[4]  T. Wilcox and R. Baillargeon, “Object individuation in infancy: the use of featural information in reasoning about occlusion events,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 97–155, 1998.
[5]  T. Wilcox and R. Baillargeon, “Object individuation in young infants: further evidence with an event-monitoring paradigm,” Developmental Science, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 127–142, 1998.
[6]  J. G. Bremner, “Cognitive development: knowledge of the physical world,” in Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Infant Development. Volume I: Basic Research, J. G. Bremner and T. D. Wachs, Eds., pp. 204–242, Wiley Blackwell, Chichester, UK, 2nd edition, 2010.
[7]  A. Needham and R. Baillargeon, “Infants' use of featural and experiential information in segregating and individuating objects: a reply to Xu, Carey and Welch (2000),” Cognition, vol. 74, no. 3, pp. 255–284, 2000.
[8]  F. Xu and S. Carey, “The emergence of kind concepts: a rejoinder to Needham and Baillargeon (2000),” Cognition, vol. 74, no. 3, pp. 285–301, 2000.
[9]  F. Xu, “The development of object individuation in infancy,” in Progress in Infancy Research, J. Fagan and H. Haynes, Eds., vol. 3, pp. 159–192, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2003.
[10]  F. Xu, S. Carey, and N. Quint, “The emergence of kind-based object individuation in infancy,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 49, no. 2, pp. 155–190, 2004.
[11]  R. Baillargeon, “Innate ideas revisited: for a principle of persistence in infants’physical reasoning,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 3, pp. 2–13, 2008.
[12]  P. Kr?jgaard, “Comparing infants' use of featural and spatiotemporal information in an object individuation task using a new event-monitoring design,” Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 6, pp. 892–909, 2007.
[13]  P. Kr?jgaard, “A direct comparison of infants' use of featural and spatiotemporal information when individuating objects in a non-occlusion event-monitoring design,” European Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 6, no. 6, pp. 641–665, 2009.
[14]  R. Brown, “How shall a thing be called?” Psychological Review, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 14–21, 1958.
[15]  M. J. Peltola, J. M. Lepp?nen, V. K. Vogel-Farley, J. K. Hietanen, and C. A. Nelson, “Fearful faces but not fearful eyes alone delay attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants,” Emotion, vol. 9, no. 4, pp. 560–565, 2009.
[16]  P. C. Quinn, M. M. Doran, J. E. Reiss, and J. E. Hoffman, “Time course of visual attention in infant categorization of cats versus dogs: evidence for a head bias as revealed through eye tracking,” Child Development, vol. 80, no. 1, pp. 151–161, 2009.
[17]  R. N. Aslin, “What's in a look?” Developmental Science, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 48–53, 2007.
[18]  R. N. Aslin, “Infant eyes: a window on cognitive development,” Infancy, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 126–140, 2012.
[19]  T. Wilcox and C. Chapa, “Priming infants to attend to color and pattern information in an individuation task,” Cognition, vol. 90, no. 3, pp. 265–302, 2004.
[20]  S. Carey and F. Xu, “Infants' knowledge of objects: beyond object files and object tracking,” Cognition, vol. 80, no. 1-2, pp. 179–213, 2001.
[21]  O. S. Kingo and P. Kr?jgaard, “Object manipulation facilitates kind-based object individuation of shape-similar objects,” Cognitive Development, vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 87–103, 2011.
[22]  S. Pauen, “Evidence for knowledge-based category discrimination in infancy,” Child Development, vol. 73, no. 4, pp. 1016–1033, 2002.
[23]  T. Wilcox, “Object individuation: infants' use of shape, size, pattern, and color,” Cognition, vol. 72, no. 2, pp. 125–166, 1999.
[24]  T. Wilcox and A. Schweinle, “Object individuation and event mapping: developmental changes in infants' use of featural information,” Developmental Science, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 132–150, 2002.
[25]  R. J. Woods, T. Wilcox, J. Armstrong, and G. Alexander, “Infants' representations of three-dimensional occluded objects,” Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 663–671, 2010.
[26]  P. Kr?jgaard, “Object individuation in 10-month-old infants: do significant objects make a difference?” Cognitive Development, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 169–184, 2000.
[27]  P. Kr?jgaard, “Object individuation in 10-month-old infants: manipulating the amount of introduction,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 447–463, 2003.
[28]  A. Aguiar and R. Baillargeon, “2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 39, no. 2, pp. 116–157, 1999.
[29]  A. Aguiar and R. Baillargeon, “Developments in young infants' reasoning about occluded objects,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 267–336, 2002.
[30]  Y. Luo and R. Baillargeon, “Can a self-propelled box have a goal?—psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants,” Psychological Science, vol. 16, no. 8, pp. 601–608, 2005.
[31]  Y. Luo and R. Baillargeon, “When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants,” Cognition, vol. 95, no. 3, pp. 297–328, 2005.
[32]  Y. Luo, L. Kaufman, and R. Baillargeon, “Young infants' reasoning about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 58, no. 4, pp. 441–486, 2009.
[33]  J. G. Bremner, S. P. Johnson, A. Slater et al., “Conditions for young infants' perception of object trajectories,” Child Development, vol. 76, no. 5, pp. 1029–1043, 2005.
[34]  D. Mareschal and M. H. Johnson, “The “what” and “where” of object representations in infancy,” Cognition, vol. 88, no. 3, pp. 259–276, 2003.
[35]  K. Shutts, L. Markson, and E. S. Spelke, “The developmental origins of animal and artifact concepts,” in The Origins of Object Knowledge, B. M. Hood and L. R. Santos, Eds., pp. 189–210, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2009.
[36]  P. J. Kellman and M. Banks, “Infant visual perception,” in Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, Perception and Language, W. Damon, D. Kuhn, and R. Siegler, Eds., pp. 103–146, Wiley, New York, NY, USA, 5th edition, 1998.
[37]  D. Y. Teller, “Spatial and temporal aspects of infant color vision,” Vision Research, vol. 38, no. 21, pp. 3275–3282, 1998.
[38]  E. S. Spelke, “Perception of unity, persistence, and identity,” in Neonate Cognition: Beyond the Blooming Buzzing Confusion, J. Mehler and R. Fox, Eds., pp. 89–113, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1985.
[39]  A. Slater and V. Morison, “Shape constancy and slant perception at birth,” Perception, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 337–344, 1985.
[40]  A. Slater, A. Mattock, and E. Brown, “Size constancy at birth: newborn infants' responses to retinal and real size,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 49, no. 2, pp. 314–322, 1990.
[41]  P. C. Quinn and P. D. Eimas, “Perceptual organization and categorization in young infants,” Advances in Infancy Research, vol. 10, pp. 1–36, 1996.
[42]  M. E. Arterberry, L. G. Craton, and A. Yonas, “Infants’ sensitivity to motion-carried information for depth and object properties,” in Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy, C. E. Granrud, Ed., pp. 215–234, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1993.
[43]  P. W. Jusczyk, S. P. Johnson, E. S. Spelke, and L. J. Kennedy, “Synchronous change and perception of object unity: evidence from adults and infants,” Cognition, vol. 71, no. 3, pp. 257–288, 1999.
[44]  L. Bonatti, E. Frot, R. Zangl, and J. Mehler, “The human first hypothesis: identification of conspecifics and individuation of objects in the young infant,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 44, no. 4, pp. 388–426, 2002.
[45]  A. M. Leslie, F. Xu, P. D. Tremoulet, and B. J. Scholl, “Indexing and the object concept: developing ‘what’ and ‘where’ systems,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 10–18, 1998.
[46]  M. M. Haith and J. B. Benson, “Infant cognition,” in Handbook of Child Psychology, W. Damon, D. Kuhn, and R. Siegler, Eds., vol. 2, pp. 199–254, Wiley, New York, NY, USA, 1998.
[47]  M. M. Haith, “Who put the cog in infant cognition? Is rich interpretation too costly?” Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 167–179, 1998.
[48]  T. Wilcox and T. R. Smith, “The development of infants' use of property-poor sounds to individuate objects,” Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 596–604, 2010.
[49]  E. S. Spelke, “Discussion, section C: the ontogeny of perceptual and causal knowledge,” in Thought without Language, L. Weiskrantz, Ed., pp. 229–236, Clarendon Press/ Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 1988.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133