全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Examining the Relative Contribution of Memory Updating, Attention Focus Switching, and Sustained Attention to Children’s Verbal Working Memory Span

DOI: 10.1155/2013/763808

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Whereas considerable developmental memory research has examined the contributions of short-term memory, processing efficiency, retention duration, and scope of attention to complex memory span, little is known about the influence of controlled attention. The present study investigated the relative influence of three understudied attention mechanisms on the verbal working memory span of school-age children: memory updating; attention focus switching; and sustained attention. Results of general linear modeling revealed that, after controlling for age, only updating accuracy emerged as a significant predictor of verbal working memory span. Memory updating speed (that subsumed attention focus switching speed) also contributed but was mediated by age. The results extend the developmental memory literature by implicating the mechanism of memory updating and developmental improvement in speed of attention focus switching and updating as critical contributors to children’s verbal working memory. Theoretically, the results provide substantively new information about the role of domain-general executive attention in children’s verbal working memory. 1. Introduction Working memory (WM) refers to a limited-capacity system that functions to encode, store, and retrieve information being processed in any cognitive task [1–6]. Working memory is conventionally measured using complex memory span tasks that are characterized by maintenance of items during processing [7, 8]. Developmental memory research indicates that children’s WM system comprises the components of short-term memory storage, processing speed [9–11], and a central executive [2, 12–20]. There are considerable developmental data focusing on the influence of storage and processing speed on children’s WM [10, 13, 21–24]. However, few studies have directly explored the contribution of attention mechanisms on WM [25]. The present study, therefore, was designed to directly investigate the contribution of attention to school-age children’s verbal WM. Working memory, functionally defined as the ability to manage information storage and retrieval during an ongoing cognitive task, requires controlled attention toward both storage and processing [13, 14, 16, 19, 26–29]. Individuals with better controlled attention are better able than those with poorer attention to maintain more items while performing a cognitive activity [19]. Cowan and colleagues have proposed that WM is defined functionally as the number of information units/chunks that can be held in the scope of attention at any given point and attention control

References

[1]  R. C. Atkinson and R. M. Shiffrin, “The control of short-term memory,” Scientific American, vol. 225, no. 2, pp. 82–90, 1971.
[2]  A. Baddeley, “Working memory and language: an overview,” Journal of Communication Disorders, vol. 36, no. 3, pp. 189–208, 2003.
[3]  N. Cowan, Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework, Oxford University Press, New York , NY, USA, 1995.
[4]  R. Engle, M. Kane, and S. Tuholski, “Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex,” in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, A. Miyake and P. Shah, Eds., pp. 102–134, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1999.
[5]  M. A. Just and P. A. Carpenter, “A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory,” Psychological Review, vol. 99, no. 1, pp. 122–149, 1992.
[6]  G. Miller, E. Gallanter, and K. Pribram, Plans and the Structure of Behavior, Holt, New York, NY, USA, 1960.
[7]  R. Case, D. M. Kurland, and J. Goldberg, “Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 33, no. 3, pp. 386–404, 1982.
[8]  M. Daneman and P. Carpenter, “Individual differences in working memory and reading,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, vol. 19, pp. 450–466, 1980.
[9]  D. M. Bayliss, C. Jarrold, D. M. Gunn, and A. D. Baddeley, “The complexities of complex span: explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 132, no. 1, pp. 71–92, 2003.
[10]  D. M. Bayliss, C. Jarrold, D. M. Gunn, A. D. Baddeley, and E. Leigh, “Mapping the developmental constraints on working memory span performance,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 41, no. 4, pp. 579–597, 2005.
[11]  R. Case, Intellectual Development: Birth to Adulthood, Academic Press, New York, NY, USA, 1985.
[12]  P. Barrouillet and V. Camos, “Developmental increase in working memory span: resource sharing or temporal decay?” Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 45, no. 1, pp. 1–20, 2001.
[13]  P. Barrouillet, N. Gavens, E. Vergauwe, V. Gaillard, and V. Camos, “Working memory span development: a time-based resource-sharing model account,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 477–490, 2009.
[14]  P. Barrouillet, S. Portrat, and V. Camos, “On the law relating processing to storage in working memory,” Psychological Review, vol. 118, no. 2, pp. 175–192, 2011.
[15]  A. R. A. Conway and R. W. Engle, “Individual differences in working memory capacity: more evidence for a general capacity theory,” Memory, vol. 4, no. 6, pp. 577–590, 1996.
[16]  N. Cowan, E. M. Elliott, S. J. Saults et al., “On the capacity of attention: its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 51, no. 1, pp. 42–100, 2005.
[17]  N. Cowan, C. C. Morey, A. M. Aubuchon, C. E. Zwilling, and A. L. Gilchrist, “Seven-year-olds allocate attention like adults unless working memory is overloaded,” Developmental Science, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 120–133, 2010.
[18]  N. Gavens and P. Barrouillet, “Delays of retention, processing efficiency, and attentional resources in working memory span development,” Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 51, no. 4, pp. 644–657, 2004.
[19]  M. J. Kane, A. R. A. Conway, M. K. Bleckley, and R. W. Engle, “A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 130, no. 2, pp. 169–183, 2001.
[20]  A. Baddeley, “Exploring the central executive,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, vol. 49, no. 1, pp. 5–28, 1996.
[21]  B. Magimairaj and J. Montgomery, “Children’s verbal working memory: relative importance of storage, general processing speed, and domain-general controlled attention,” Acta Psychologica, vol. 140, pp. 196–207, 2012.
[22]  J. N. Towse and G. J. Hitch, “Is there a relationship between task demand and storage space in tests of working memory capacity?” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, vol. 48, no. 1, pp. 108–124, 1995.
[23]  J. N. Towse, G. J. Hitch, and U. Hutton, “A re-evaluation of working memory capacity in children,” Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 39, no. 2, pp. 195–217, 1998.
[24]  J. N. Towse, G. J. Hitch, and U. Hutton, “On the nature of the relationship between processing activity and item retention in children,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 82, no. 2, pp. 156–184, 2002.
[25]  H. L. St Clair-Thompson and S. E. Gathercole, “Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory,” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 745–759, 2006.
[26]  P. Barrouillet and V. Camos, “Working memory and executive control: a time-based resource-sharing account,” Psychologica Belgica, vol. 50, no. 3-4, pp. 353–382, 2010.
[27]  N. Cowan, “An embedded-process model of working memory,” in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, A. Miyake and P. Shah , Eds., pp. 62–101, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1999.
[28]  R. W. Engle, J. Cantor, and J. J. Carullo, “Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: a test of four hypotheses,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 18, no. 5, pp. 972–992, 1992.
[29]  M. L. Turner and R. W. Engle, “Is working memory capacity task dependent?” Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 127–154, 1989.
[30]  P. Barrouillet, S. Bernardin, and V. Camos, “Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 133, no. 1, pp. 83–100, 2004.
[31]  C. Zoelch, K. Seitz, and R. Schumann-Hengsteler, “From rag(bag)s to riches: measuring the developing central executive,” in Young Children’s Cognitive Development: Inter-Relationships among Executive Functioning, Working Memory, Verbal Ability and Theory of Mind, W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, and B. Sodian, Eds., pp. 39–70, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005.
[32]  N. Cowan, L. D. Nugent, E. M. Elliott, I. Ponomarev, and J. S. Saults, “The role of attention in the development of short-term memory: age differences in the verbal span of apprehension,” Child Development, vol. 70, no. 5, pp. 1082–1097, 1999.
[33]  S. Portrat, V. Camos, and P. Barrouillet, “Working memory in children: a time-constrained functioning similar to adults,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 102, no. 3, pp. 368–374, 2009.
[34]  H. Irwin-Chase and B. Burns, “Developmental changes in children’s abilities to share and allocate attention in a dual task,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 77, no. 1, pp. 61–85, 2000.
[35]  C. Karatekin, “Development of attentional allocation in the dual task paradigm,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 52, no. 1, pp. 7–21, 2004.
[36]  C. Karatekin, J. W. Couperus, and D. J. Marcus, “Attention allocation in the dual-task paradigm as measured through behavioral and psychophysiological responses,” Psychophysiology, vol. 41, no. 2, pp. 175–185, 2004.
[37]  A. Diamond, “Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 21, no. 5, pp. 335–341, 2012.
[38]  K. Ebert, J. Rentmeester-Disher, and K. Kohnert, “Nonlinguistic cognitive treatment for bilingual children with primary language impairment,” Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, vol. 26, pp. 485–501, 2012.
[39]  T. Klingberg, E. Fernell, P. J. Olesen et al., “Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—a randomized, controlled trial,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 177–186, 2005.
[40]  H. St Clair-Thompson, R. Stevens, A. Hunt, and E. Bolder, “Improving children's working memory and classroom performance,” Educational Psychology, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 203–219, 2010.
[41]  L. B. Thorell, S. Lindqvist, S. B. Nutley, G. Bohlin, and T. Klingberg, “Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children,” Developmental Science, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 106–113, 2009.
[42]  Z. Shipstead, T. S. Redick, and R. W. Engle, “Does working memory training generalize?” Psychologica Belgica, vol. 50, no. 3-4, pp. 245–276, 2010.
[43]  Z. Shipstead, T. Redick, and R. Engle, “Is working memory training effective?” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 138, pp. 628–654, 2012.
[44]  N. Cowan, J. Rouder, C. Blume, and J. Saults, “Models of verbal working memory capacity: what does it take to make them work?” Psychological Review, vol. 119, no. 3, pp. 480–499, 2012.
[45]  E. Awh, E. K. Vogel, and S. H. Oh, “Interactions between attention and working memory,” Neuroscience, vol. 139, no. 1, pp. 201–208, 2006.
[46]  A. R. A. Conway and R. W. Engle, “Working memory and retrieval: a resource-dependent inhibition model,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 123, no. 4, pp. 354–373, 1994.
[47]  L. Hasher, C. Lustig, and R. Zacks, “Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention,” in Variation in Working Memory, A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, and J. Towse, Eds., pp. 227–249, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2007.
[48]  R. Lépine, S. Bernardin, and P. Barrouillet, “Attention switching and working memory spans,” European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 329–345, 2005.
[49]  N. Unsworth and R. W. Engle, “Speed and accuracy of accessing information in working memory: an individual differences investigation of focus switching,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 616–630, 2008.
[50]  N. Morris and D. Jones, “Memory updating in working memory: the role of the central executive,” British Journal of Psychology, vol. 81, pp. 111–121, 1990.
[51]  A. R. A. Conway, M. J. Kane, M. F. Bunting, D. Z. Hambrick, O. Wilhelm, and R. W. Engle, “Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user's guide,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, vol. 12, no. 5, pp. 769–786, 2005.
[52]  W. K. Kirchner, “Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 55, no. 4, pp. 352–358, 1958.
[53]  J. F. Mackworth, “Paced memorizing in a continuous task,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 58, no. 3, pp. 206–211, 1959.
[54]  I. Pollack, L. B. Johnson, and P. R. Knaff, “Running memory span,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 57, no. 3, pp. 137–146, 1959.
[55]  N. C. Waugh, “Serial position and the memory-span,” The American Journal of Psychology, vol. 73, pp. 68–79, 1960.
[56]  F. Schmiedek, A. Hildebrandt, M. L?vdén, O. Wilhelm, and U. Lindenberger, “Complex span versus updating tasks of working memory: the gap is not that deep,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 1089–1096, 2009.
[57]  N. J. Cepeda, A. F. Kramer, and J. C. Gonzalez de Sather, “Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 37, no. 5, pp. 715–730, 2001.
[58]  P. Verhaeghen and W. J. Hoyer, “Aging, focus switching, and task switching in a continuous calculation task: evidence toward a new working memory control process,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 22–39, 2007.
[59]  D. A. Pearson and D. M. Lane, “Auditory attention switching: a developmental study,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 51, no. 2, pp. 320–334, 1991.
[60]  J. Egeland and I. Kovalik-Gran, “Measuring several aspects of attention in one test: the factor structure of conners's continuous performance test,” Journal of Attention Disorders, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 339–346, 2010.
[61]  R. A. Hanson and J. W. Montgomery, “Effects of general processing capacity and sustained selective attention on temporal processing performance of children with specific language impairment,” Applied Psycholinguistics, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 75–93, 2002.
[62]  L. B. Jones, M. K. Rothbart, and M. I. Posner, “Development of executive attention in preschool children,” Developmental Science, vol. 6, no. 5, pp. 498–504, 2003.
[63]  T. Manly, V. Anderson, I. Nimmo-Smith, A. Turner, P. Watson, and I. H. Robertson, “The differential assessment of children's attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 42, no. 8, pp. 1065–1081, 2001.
[64]  M. Noterdaeme, H. Amorosa, K. Mildenberger, S. Sitter, and F. Minow, “Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder,” European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 58–66, 2001.
[65]  T. J. Spaulding, E. Plante, and R. Vance, “Sustained selective attention skills of preschool children with specific language impairment: evidence for separate attentional capacities,” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 51, no. 1, pp. 16–34, 2008.
[66]  J. Jonides, S. C. Lacey, and D. E. Nee, “Processes of working memory in mind and brain,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 2–5, 2005.
[67]  S. E. Gathercole, “Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 3, no. 11, pp. 410–419, 1999.
[68]  R. Morris, “Relationships and distinctions among the concepts of attention, memory, and executive function: a developmental perspective,” in Attention, Memory, and Executive Function, G. Lyon and N. Krasnegor, Eds., pp. 11–16, Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, Md, USA, 1996.
[69]  L. Brown, R. Sherbenou, and S. Johnsen, Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-3, Pro-ed, Austin, Tex, USA, 1997.
[70]  American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Guidelines for Audiologic Screening [Guidelines], 1997, http://www.asha.org/policy/.
[71]  E. Semel, E. Wiig, and W. Secord, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 2003.
[72]  D. Bishop, Test for Reception of Grammar-2, The Psychological Corporation, London, UK, 2003.
[73]  L. Dunn and L. Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 3, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minn, USA, 1997.
[74]  W. Schneider, A. Eschmann, and A. Zuccolotto, E-Prime User’s Guide, Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pa, USA, 2002.
[75]  H. Garavan, “Serial attention within working memory,” Memory and Cognition, vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 263–276, 1998.
[76]  H. Garavan, T. J. Ross, S. J. Li, and E. A. Stein, “A parametric manipulation of central executive functioning,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 10, no. 6, pp. 585–592, 2000.
[77]  M. C. Davidson, D. Amso, L. C. Anderson, and A. Diamond, “Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 44, no. 11, pp. 2037–2078, 2006.
[78]  M. Huizinga and M. W. van der Molen, “Task switching and shifting between stopping and going: developmental change in between-trial control adjustments,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 484–503, 2011.
[79]  M. Gordon, F. McClure, and G. Aylward, The Gordon Diagnostic System: Instructional Manual and Interpretive Guide, Gordon Systems, New York, NY, USA, 3rd edition, 1996.
[80]  C. Riccio, C. Reynolds, and P. Lowe, Clinical Applications of Continuous Performance Tests: Measuring Attention and Impulsive Responding in Children, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA, 2001.
[81]  D. M. Green and J. A. Swets, Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA, 1966.
[82]  R. E. Pastore and C. J. Scheirer, “Signal detection theory: considerations for general application,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 81, no. 12, pp. 945–958, 1974.
[83]  B. Magimairaj, J. Montgomery, S. Marinellie, and J. McCarthy, “Relation of three mechanisms of working memory to children's complex span performance,” International Journal of Behavioral Development, vol. 33, no. 5, pp. 460–469, 2009.
[84]  J. R. Booth, B. MacWhinney, and Y. Harasaki, “Developmental differences in visual and auditory processing of complex sentences,” Child Development, vol. 71, no. 4, pp. 981–1003, 2000.
[85]  F. Dick, B. Wulfeck, M. Krupa-Kwiatkowski, and E. Bates, “The development of complex sentence interpretation in typically developing children compared with children with specific language impairments or early unilateral focal lesions,” Developmental Science, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 360–377, 2004.
[86]  B. MacWhinney, “Basic syntactic processes,” in Language Acquisition, S. Kuczaj, Ed., vol. 1 of Syntax and Semantics, pp. 73–122, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1982.
[87]  M. J. Cortese and M. M. Khanna, “Age of acquisition ratings for 3,000 monosyllabic words,” Behavior Research Methods, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 791–794, 2008.
[88]  A. Moe, C. Hopkins, and R. Rush, The Vocabulary of First-Grade Children, Thomas, Springfield, Ill, USA.
[89]  S. E. Weismer and L. J. Hesketh, “The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment,” Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, vol. 36, no. 5, pp. 1013–1025, 1993.
[90]  S. Pickering and S. Gathercole, Working Memory Test Battery for Children, The Psychological Corporation, London, UK, 2001.
[91]  N. P. Friedman and A. Miyake, “The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent variable analysis,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 133, no. 1, pp. 101–135, 2004.
[92]  R. W. Engle, J. E. Laughlin, S. W. Tuholski, and A. R. A. Conway, “Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 128, no. 3, pp. 309–331, 1999.
[93]  J. Towse and C. Houston-Price, “Reflections of the concept of the central executive,” in Working Memory in Perspective, J. Andrade, Ed., pp. 240–260, Psychology Press, New York, NY, USA, 2001.
[94]  N. Cowan, “Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 104, no. 2, pp. 163–191, 1988.
[95]  B. McElree, “Working memory and focal attention,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 817–835, 2001.
[96]  N. Unsworth and R. W. Engle, “On the division of short-term and working memory: an examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 133, no. 6, pp. 1038–1066, 2007.
[97]  K. Oberauer, “Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 28, no. 3, pp. 411–421, 2002.
[98]  K. Oberauer, “Selective attention to elements in working memory,” Experimental Psychology, vol. 50, no. 4, pp. 257–269, 2003.
[99]  P. Verhaeghen and C. Basak, “Ageing and switching of the focus of attention in working memory: results from a modified N-Back task,” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, vol. 58, no. 1, pp. 134–154, 2005.
[100]  J. Towse and N. Cowan, “Working memory and its relevance for cognitive development,” in Young Children’s Cognitive Development: Inter-Relationships among Executive Functioning, Working Memory, Verbal Ability and Theory of Mind, W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, and B. Sodian, Eds., pp. 9–38, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005.
[101]  J. May, “Specifying the central executive may require complexity,” in Working Memory in Perspective, J. Andrade, Ed., pp. 261–277, Psychology Press, New York, NY, USA, 2001.
[102]  V. Gaillard, P. Barrouillet, C. Jarrold, and V. Camos, “Developmental differences in working memory: where do they come from?” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 110, no. 3, pp. 469–479, 2011.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413