全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

An Association between Emotional Responsiveness and Smoking Behavior

DOI: 10.1155/2013/276024

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Introduction. Emotional responsiveness (ER) has been theorized to play a protective role in pathways to tobacco initiation, regular use, and dependence, yet a possible association between ER and smoking behavior has not been studied. Our aim was to test whether measuring ER to a neutral stimulus was associated with decreased odds of current smoking. Methods. We measured ER and smoking status (current, former, and never) in two datasets: a cross-sectional dataset of persons with diabetes ( ) and a prospective dataset of depressed patients ( ) from an urban primary care system. Because there were few former smokers in the datasets, smoking status was dichotomized (current versus former/never) and measured at baseline (cross-sectional dataset) or at 36 weeks after-baseline (prospective dataset). ER was ascertained with response to a neutral facial expression (any ER versus none). Results. Compared to their nonresponsive counterparts, adjusted odds of current smoking were lower among participants endorsing emotional responsiveness in both the cross-sectional and prospective datasets (ORs = .29 and .32, ’s , resp.). Discussion. ER may be protective against current smoking behavior. Further research investigating the association between ER and decreased smoking may hold potential to inform treatment approaches to improve smoking prevalence. 1. Introduction In the United States about 20% of persons aged 16 and older report smoking, and smoking rates are higher among persons from lower socioeconomic strata [1]. While a range of treatments exist to help smokers quit, among persons from lower socioeconomic groups the evidence that interventions increase cessation is sparse [2]. Elucidation of novel factors associated with smoking behavior holds potential to substantively improve understanding of who experiments with tobacco, who becomes a regular user, or who successfully quits. Moreover, uncovering such person-level factors may inform adjustments to treatment approaches that prevent initiation, decrease prevalence of regular tobacco use, and support cessation. Person-level characteristics associated with smoking behavior are generally classified as psychopathology, personality, or gene related. Depressive symptoms, anxiety, psychosis, anger, social alienation, impulsivity, sensation seeking tendency, and attentional dysfunction have all been associated with current smoking [3]. Neurotic, extraverted, and open personality characteristics are associated with lifetime tobacco use [4]. Genetic variations in the nicotinic and dopamine receptors have been associated

References

[1]  J. Sargeant, M. Valli, S. Ferrier, and H. MacLeod, “Lifestyle counseling in primary care: opportunities and challenges for changing practice,” Medical Teacher, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 185–191, 2008.
[2]  R. Hiscock, L. Bauld, A. Amos, J. A. Fidler, and M. Munafò, “Socioeconomic status and smoking: a review,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1248, no. 1, pp. 107–123, 2012.
[3]  D. G. Gilbert and B. O. Gilbert, “Personality, psychopathology, and nicotine response as mediators of the genetics of smoking,” Behavior Genetics, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 133–147, 1995.
[4]  C. W. Kahler, A. M. Leventhal, S. B. Daughters et al., “Relationships of personality and psychiatric disorders to multiple domains of smoking motives and dependence in middle-aged adults,” Nicotine and Tobacco Research, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 381–389, 2010.
[5]  L. J. Bierut, “Nicotine dependence and genetic variation in the nicotinic receptors,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 104, supplement 1, pp. S64–S69, 2009.
[6]  A. M. Leventhal, S. P. David, M. Brightman et al., “Dopamine D4 receptor gene variation moderates the efficacy of bupropion for smoking cessation,” Pharmacogenomics Journal, vol. 12, pp. 86–92, 2010.
[7]  S. P. David, E. C. Johnstone, M. Churchman, P. Aveyard, M. F. G. Murphy, and M. R. Munafò, “Pharmacogenetics of smoking cessation in general practice: results from the Patch II and Patch in Practice trials,” Nicotine and Tobacco Research, vol. 13, no. 3, pp. 157–167, 2011.
[8]  C. W. Kahler, S. B. Daughters, A. M. Leventhal et al., “Personality, psychiatric disorders, and smoking in middle-aged adults,” Nicotine and Tobacco Research, vol. 11, no. 7, pp. 833–841, 2009.
[9]  M. R. Munafò, J. I. Zetteler, and T. G. Clark, “Personality and smoking status: a meta-analysis,” Nicotine and Tobacco Research, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 405–413, 2007.
[10]  R. West and A. Hardy, Theory of Addiction, Blackwell, Oxford, UK, 2006.
[11]  R. D. Keeley, A. J. Davidson, L. A. Crane, B. Matthews, and W. Pace, “An association between negatively biased response to neutral stimuli and antidepressant nonadherence,” Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 62, no. 5, pp. 535–544, 2007.
[12]  J. Lee, R. Keeley, and A. Reiter, “Novel correlates of coronary heart disease risk,” Journal of Bioscience and Medicine, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 1–10, 2012.
[13]  M. L. Phillips, W. C. Drevets, S. L. Rauch, and R. Lane, “Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 54, no. 5, pp. 515–528, 2003.
[14]  R. M. Nesse and P. C. Ellsworth, “Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders,” American Psychologist, vol. 64, no. 2, pp. 129–139, 2009.
[15]  P. Ekman and W. V. Friesen, Emotion in the Human Face, Pergamon, Elmsford, NY, USA, 1972.
[16]  D. L. Patrick, A. Cheadle, D. C. Thompson, P. Diehr, T. Koepsell, and S. Kinne, “The validity of self-reported smoking: a review and meta-analysis,” American Journal of Public Health, vol. 84, no. 7, pp. 1086–1093, 1994.
[17]  O. P. John and E. M. Donahue, The Big Five Inventory: Construction and Validation, Institute of Personality and Social Research, Berkeley, Calif, USA, 1998.
[18]  M. L. Phillips, W. C. Drevets, S. L. Rauch, and R. Lane, “Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 54, no. 5, pp. 504–514, 2003.
[19]  J. M. Lepp?nen, M. Milders, J. S. Bell, E. Terriere, and J. K. Hietanen, “Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 128, no. 2, pp. 123–133, 2004.
[20]  W. Luo, W. Feng, W. He, N. Y. Wang, and Y. J. Luo, “Three stages of facial expression processing: ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation,” Neuroimage, vol. 49, no. 2, pp. 1857–1867, 2010.
[21]  J. Joormann, K. Gilbert, and I. H. Gotlib, “Emotion identification in girls at high risk for depression,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 51, no. 5, pp. 575–582, 2010.
[22]  G. H. De Weert-Van Oene, C. A. J. De Jong, F. J?rg, and G. J. P. Schrijvers, “The helping alliance questionnaire: psychometric properties in patients with substance dependence,” Substance Use and Misuse, vol. 34, no. 11, pp. 1549–1569, 1999.
[23]  M. Jerusalem and R. Schwarzer, “Self-efficacy as a resource factor in stress appraisal processes,” in Self-Efficacy: Thought Control of Action, R. Schwarzer, Ed., pp. 195–213, Hemisphere, Washington, DC, USA, 1992.
[24]  S. Schneider, M. Gadinger, and A. Fischer, “Does the effect go up in smoke? A randomized controlled trial of pictorial warnings on cigarette packaging,” Patient Education and Counseling, vol. 86, no. 1, pp. 77–83, 2012.
[25]  C. A. McHorney, J. E. Ware, J. F. Lu, and C. D. Sherbourne, “The MOS 36-item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): III. Tests of data quality, scaling assumptions, and reliability across diverse patient groups,” Medical Care, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 40–66, 1994.
[26]  B. L?we, K. Kroenke, and K. Gr?fe, “Detecting and monitoring depression with a two-item questionnaire (PHQ-2),” Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 58, no. 2, pp. 163–171, 2005.
[27]  J. D. Kassel, L. R. Stroud, and C. A. Paronis, “Smoking, stress, and negative affect: correlation, causation, and context across stages of smoking,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 129, no. 2, pp. 270–304, 2003.
[28]  T. N. Weingarten, Y. Shi, C. B. Mantilla, W. M. Hooten, and D. O. Warner, “Smoking and chronic pain: a real-but-puzzling relationship,” Minnesota Medicine, vol. 94, no. 3, pp. 35–37, 2011.
[29]  T. W. Strine, A. H. Mokdad, S. R. Dube et al., “The association of depression and anxiety with obesity and unhealthy behaviors among community-dwelling US adults,” General Hospital Psychiatry, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 127–137, 2008.
[30]  H. C. Kraemer and D. J. Kupfer, “Size of treatment effects and their importance to clinical research and practice,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 59, no. 11, pp. 990–996, 2006.
[31]  B. Chapman, K. Fiscella, P. Duberstein, and I. Kawachi, “Education and smoking: confounding or effect modification by phenotypic personality traits?” Annals of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 237–248, 2009.
[32]  Y. Shi, T. N. Weingarten, C. B. Mantilla, W. M. Hooten, and D. O. Warner, “Smoking and pain: pathophysiology and clinical implications,” Anesthesiology, vol. 113, no. 4, pp. 977–992, 2010.
[33]  L. G. Escobedo, R. F. Anda, P. F. Smith, P. L. Remington, and E. E. Mast, “Sociodemographic characteristics of cigarette smoking initiation in the United States: implications for smoking prevention policy,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 264, no. 12, pp. 1550–1555, 1990.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413