全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Everyday Prospective Memory and Executive Function Deficits Associated with Exposure to Second-Hand Smoke

DOI: 10.1155/2013/160486

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

This study explored whether exposure to second-hand smoke (SHS) has a detrimental impact upon everyday memory in two groups of non-smokers; one which reported regular exposure to SHS and one that reported never having been exposed to SHS. Thirty-four non-smokers who reported having been regularly exposed to SHS (SHS group) and 34 non-smokers who reported never having been exposed to SHS (non-SHS group) were compared on self-reports of prospective memory (PM: remembering future intentions and/or activities) and executive function (EF: those processes involved in attention, multitasking and decision-making). The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) assessed everyday PM lapses; the Executive Function Questionnaire (EFQ) assessed self-reported problems in EF; a drug-use questionnaire and a mood questionnaire were also administered. Two univariate ANCOVAs were applied to the PM and EF data, controlling for between-group differences in age, weekly alcohol use, anxiety and depression scores, and self-reported retrospective memory scores. The SHS group reported significantly more lapses on the PRMQ and more deficits on the EFQ than the non-SHS group. These findings provide new insights into PM and EF deficits associated with prolonged exposure to SHS in a group of non-smokers. Possible explanations and suggestions for future research are also considered. 1. Introduction Second-hand smoke (SHS) refers to a situation where one person inhales another person’s smoke either by exposure to side stream smoke (smoke emitted from the end of a cigarette, pipe, or cigar) or mainstream smoke (the smoke that is exhaled by the smoker directly). Previous research has suggested that exposure to second-hand smoke (SHS) not only has a detrimental effect upon health, including cardiovascular disease [1, 2], but also is associated with poorer cognitive performance in children, adolescents, and adults (3–6). For example, children exposed to SHS show reduced vocabulary and reasoning abilities [3], as well as more general cognitive and intellectual deficits [4]. In addition, recent work has shown a strong relationship between exposure to SHS and impairments in reading, mathematics, and visuospatial skills in children and adolescents [5] and poorer cognitive function in adults [6, 7]. In the first of these adult studies [6], participants included in the study had no history of smoking or using any tobacco product, and had no history of cardiovascular disease or dementia. Based on their self-reported long-term exposure to SHS tobacco smoke (having lived with a

References

[1]  D. F. Dietrich, J. Schwartz, C. Schindler et al., “Effects of passive smoking on heart rate variability, heart rate and blood pressure: an observational study,” International Journal of Epidemiology, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 834–840, 2007.
[2]  M. D. Eisner, Y. Wang, T. J. Haight, J. Balmes, S. K. Hammond, and I. B. Tager, “Secondhand smoke exposure, pulmonary function, and cardiovascular mortality,” Annals of Epidemiology, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 364–373, 2007.
[3]  B. Eskenazi and J. J. Bergmann, “Passive and active maternal smoking during pregnancy, as measured by serum cotinine, and postnatal smoke exposure. I. Effects on physical growth at age 5 years,” American Journal of Epidemiology, vol. 142, no. 9, supplement, pp. S10–S18, 1995.
[4]  K. E. Bauman, R. L. Flewelling, and J. LaPrelle, “Parental cigarette smoking and cognitive performance of children,” Health Psychology, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 282–288, 1991.
[5]  K. Yolton, K. Dietrich, P. Auinger, B. P. Lanphear, and R. Hornung, “Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among U.S. children and adolescents,” Environmental Health Perspectives, vol. 113, no. 1, pp. 98–103, 2005.
[6]  M. J. Friedrich, “Researchers report new clues to dementia,” The Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 298, no. 2, pp. 161–163, 2007.
[7]  D. J. Llewellyn, L. A. Lang, K. M. Langa, F. Naughton, and F. E. Matthews, “Exposure to secondhand smoke and cognitive impairment in non-smokers: national cross sectional study with cotinine measurement,” British Medical Journal, vol. 338, no. 7695, pp. 1–6, 2009.
[8]  K. K. Teo, S. Ounpuu, S. Hawken et al., “Tobacco use and risk of myocardial infarction in 52 countries in the INTERHEART study: a case-control study,” The Lancet, vol. 368, no. 9536, pp. 647–658, 2006.
[9]  A. Penn, L. C. Chen, and C. A. Snyder, “Inhalation of steady-state sidestream smoke from one cigarette promotes arteriosclerotic plaque development,” Circulation, vol. 90, no. 3, pp. 1363–1367, 1994.
[10]  M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, and M. A. McDaniel, Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New York, NY, USA, 1996.
[11]  A. Baddeley, “Working memory: looking back and looking forward,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 4, no. 10, pp. 829–839, 2003.
[12]  A. D. Baddeley and G. J. Hitch, “Developments in the concept of working memory,” Neuropsychology, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 485–493, 1994.
[13]  M. A. McDaniel and G. O. Einstein, Prospective Memory: An Overview and Synthesis of an Emerging Field, Sage, London, UK, 2007.
[14]  P. S. Bisiacchi, “The neuropsychological approach in the study of prospective memory,” in Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications, M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, and M. A. McDaniel, Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New York, NY, USA, 1996.
[15]  P. W. Burgess, E. Veitch, A. de Lacy Costello, and T. Shallice, “The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 38, no. 6, pp. 848–863, 2000.
[16]  C. A. Johnson, L. Xiao, P. Palmer et al., “Affective decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in 10th grade Chinese adolescent binge drinkers,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 714–726, 2008.
[17]  M. A. McDaniel, E. L. Glisky, S. R. Rubin, M. J. Guynn, and B. C. Routhieaux, “Prospective memory: a neuropsychological study,” Neuropsychology, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 103–110, 1999.
[18]  E. K. Miller and J. D. Wallis, “Executive function and higher-order cognition: definition and neural substrates,” in Encyclopaedia of Neuroscience, L. J. Squire, Ed., vol. 4, pp. 99–104, Academic Press, Oxford, UK, 2009.
[19]  M. Kliegel, T. Jager, M. Altgassen, and D. Shum, “Clinical neuropsychology of prospective memory,” in Prospective Memory: Cognitive, Neuroscience, Developmental and Applied Perspectives, M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, and G. O. Einstein, Eds., pp. 283–308, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2008.
[20]  M. Martin, M. Kliegel, and M. A. McDaniel, “The involvement of executive functions in prospective memory performance of adults,” International Journal of Psychology, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 195–206, 2003.
[21]  A. P. Shimamura, J. S. Janowsky, and L. R. Squire, “What is the role of frontal lobe damage in memory disorders?” in Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction, H. S. Levin, E. M. Eisenberg, and A. L. Benton, Eds., pp. 173–195, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1991.
[22]  P. W. Burgess, A. Quayle, and C. D. Frith, “Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 39, no. 6, pp. 545–555, 2001.
[23]  World Health Organisation, WHO Report on the Global Tobacco Epidemic, 2009, World Health Organisation, Geneva, Switzerland, 2009.
[24]  T. M. Heffernan, “The impact of excessive alcohol use on prospective memory: a brief review,” Current Drug Abuse Reviews, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 36–41, 2008.
[25]  J. Rodgers, T. Buchanan, A. B. Scholey, T. M. Heffernan, J. Ling, and A. C. Parrott, “Prospective memory: the influence of ecstasy, cannabis and nicotine use and the WWW,” The Open Addiction Journal, vol. 4, pp. 44–45, 2011.
[26]  A. C. Parrott, A. Morinan, M. Moss, and A. Scholey, Understanding Drugs and Behaviour, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, UK, 2004.
[27]  R. Antikainen, T. H?nninen, K. Honkalampi et al., “Mood improvement reduces memory complaints in depressed patients,” European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 251, no. 1, pp. 6–11, 2001.
[28]  A. S. Zigmond and R. P. Snaith, “The hospital anxiety and depression scale,” Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, vol. 67, no. 6, pp. 361–370, 1983.
[29]  J. R. Crawford, J. D. Henry, C. Crombie, and E. P. Taylor, “Normative data for the HADS from a large non-clinical sample,” British Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 40, no. 4, pp. 429–434, 2001.
[30]  J. R. Crawford, G. Smith, E. A. Maylor, S. Della Sala, and R. H. Logie, “The prospective and retrospective memory questionnaire (PRMQ): normative data and latent structure in a large non-clinical sample,” Memory, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 261–275, 2003.
[31]  T. Buchanan, T. M. Heffernan, A. C. Parrott, J. Ling, J. Rodgers, and A. B. Scholey, “A short self-report measure of problems with executive function suitable for administration via the internet,” Behavior Research Methods, vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 709–714, 2010.
[32]  T. M. Pronk, J. C. Karremans, and D. H. J. Wigboldus, “How can you resist? Executive control helps romantically involved individuals to stay faithful,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 100, no. 5, pp. 827–837, 2011.
[33]  B. A. Wilson, H. Emslie, J. Foley, A. Shiel, P. Watson, and K. Hawkins, The Cambridge Prospective Memory Test, Harcourt-Assessment, London, UK, 2005.
[34]  F. Andelman, E. Zuckerman-Feldhay, D. Hoffien, I. Fried, and M. Y. Neufeld, “Lateralization of deficit in self-awareness of memory in patients with intractable epilepsy,” Epilepsia, vol. 45, no. 7, pp. 826–833, 2004.
[35]  S. Sevush and N. Leve, “Denial of memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 150, no. 5, pp. 748–751, 1993.
[36]  D. Ghosh, M. K. Mishra, S. Das, D. K. Kaushik, and A. Basu, “Tobacco carcinogen induces microglial activation and subsequent neuronal damage,” Journal of Neurochemistry, vol. 110, no. 3, pp. 1070–1081, 2009.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413