全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

The Cross-Race Effect: Resistant to Instructions

DOI: 10.1155/2013/745836

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

The cross-race effect (CRE) is the tendency for eyewitnesses to be better at recognizing members of their own race/ethnicity than members of other races/ethnicities. It manifests in terms of both better discrimination (i.e., telling apart previously seen from new targets) and a more conservative response criterion for own-race than for other-race faces. The CRE is quite robust and generally resistant to change. Two studies examined the effectiveness of reducing the CRE with special instructions given prior to retrieval. Although instructions at retrieval did change participants’ response criterion—making them less likely to identify test faces as previously seen—they did not shift their response criterion selectively for other-race faces. The findings indirectly support the importance of encoding processes in producing the CRE. 1. Introduction There are numerous times in our criminal justice system that eyewitness testimony can make the difference between conviction and acquittal. When trials contain eyewitness testimony, jurors rely on it heavily, despite holding some erroneous beliefs about the factors that make eyewitnesses more or less accurate [1]. Because jurors rely on those beliefs in evaluating eyewitness credibility and making trial judgments [1–3], false convictions in eyewitness cases are not uncommon. Indeed, eyewitness misidentifications lead to more wrongful convictions than all other causes combined [4, 5]. One common cause of eyewitness error is the cross-race effect (CRE; also referred to as the own-race bias), which is the tendency to be worse at recognizing individuals from other racial/ethnic groups than one’s own racial/ethnic group [6–8]. The bias is quite robust; it appears in early infancy [9], has been observed in young children [10], and persists into old age [11]. Despite the robustness of the effect, people are not necessarily aware of it; for example, Abshire and Bornstein [12] found that fewer than 50% of mock jurors correctly answered a question about the CRE although Black participants were more knowledgeable than White participants. The reasons underlying the CRE are not clear. There are three leading classes of explanations for the effect, which focus on differential experience, encoding, and retrieval (for review, see [7, 13]). The experience-based explanation, often referred to as the contact hypothesis, emphasizes the differential contact that one has with one’s own versus other races. The contact hypothesis posits that the degree of contact with members of a group (i.e., quantity and quality) dictates a person’s

References

[1]  M. Boyce, J. L. Beaudry, and R. C. L. Lindsay, “Belief of eyewitness identification evidence,” in Memory for People, R. C. L. Lindsay, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, and M. P. Toglia, Eds., vol. 2 of Handbook of Eyewitness Psychology, pp. 501–525, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007.
[2]  B. H. Bornstein, S. O'Bryant, and D. Zickafoose, “Intuitions about arousal and eyewitness memory: effects on mock jurors' judgments,” Law and Psychology Review, vol. 32, pp. 109–133, 2008.
[3]  T. M. S. Neal, A. Christiansen, B. H. Bornstein, and T. R. Robicheaux, “The effects of mock jurors' beliefs about eyewitness performance on trial judgments,” Psychology, Crime and Law, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 49–64, 2012.
[4]  G. L. Wells, A. Memon, and S. D. Penrod, “Eyewitness evidence: improving its probative value,” Psychological Science in the Public Interest, Supplement, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 45–75, 2006.
[5]  G. L. Wells, M. Small, S. Penrod, R. S. Malpass, S. M. Fulero, and C. A. E. Brimacombe, “Eyewitness identification procedures: recommendations for lineups and photospreads,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 22, no. 6, pp. 603–647, 1998.
[6]  J. C. Brigham, “The role of race and racial prejudice in recognizing other people,” in Motivational Aspects of Prejudice and Racism, C. Willis-Esqueda, Ed., pp. 68–110, Springer, New York, NY, USA, 2008.
[7]  J. C. Brigham, L. B. Bennett, C. A. Meissner, and T. L. Mitchell, “The influence of race on eyewitness memory,” in Memory for People, R. C. L. Lindsay, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, and M. P. Toglia, Eds., vol. 2 of Handbook of Eyewitness Psychology, pp. 257–282, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007.
[8]  C. A. Meissner and J. C. Brigham, “Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: a meta-analytic review,” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 3–35, 2001.
[9]  D. J. Kelly, S. Liu, L. Ge et al., “Cross-race preferences for same-race faces extend beyond the african versus caucasian contrast in 3-month-old infants,” Infancy, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 87–95, 2007.
[10]  K. Pezdek, I. Blandon-Gitlin, and C. Moore, “Children's face recognition memory: more evidence for the cross-race effect,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 88, no. 4, pp. 760–763, 2003.
[11]  J. C. Brigham and N. L. Williamson, “Cross-racial recognition and age: when you're over 60, do they still ‘all look alike?’,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 5, pp. 218–222, 1979.
[12]  J. Abshire and B. H. Bornstein, “Juror sensitivity to the cross-race effect,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 471–480, 2003.
[13]  S. L. Sporer, “Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: an integration of theories,” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 36–97, 2001.
[14]  J. C. Brigham and R. S. Malpass, “The role of experience and contact in the recognition of faces of own- and other- race persons,” Journal of Social Issues, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 139–155, 1985.
[15]  P. Chiroro and T. Valentine, “An investigation of the contact hypothesis of the own-race bias in face recognition,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, vol. 48, pp. 879–894, 1995.
[16]  O. H. MacLin, B. R. van Sickler, M. K. MacLin, and A. Li, “A re-examination of the cross-race effect: the role of race, inversion, and basketball trivia,” North American Journal of Psychology, vol. 6, pp. 189–204, 2004.
[17]  K. J. Susa, C. A. Meissner, and H. D. Heer, “Modeling the role of social-cognitive processes in the recognition of own-and other-race faces,” Social Cognition, vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 523–537, 2010.
[18]  W. Ng and R. C. L. Lindsay, “Cross-Race facial recognition: failure of the contact hypothesis,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, vol. 25, pp. 217–232, 1994.
[19]  J. R. Evans, J. L. Marcon, and C. A. Meissner, “Cross-racial lineup identification: assessing the potential benefits of context reinstatement,” Psychology, Crime and Law, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 19–28, 2009.
[20]  K. Hugenberg, J. Miller, and H. M. Claypool, “Categorization and individuation in the cross-race recognition deficit: toward a solution to an insidious problem,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 43, no. 2, pp. 334–340, 2007.
[21]  C. A. Meissner, J. C. Brigham, and D. A. Butz, “Memory for own- and other-race faces: a dual-process approach,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 19, no. 5, pp. 545–567, 2005.
[22]  D. T. Levin, “Race as a visual feature: using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol. 129, no. 4, pp. 559–574, 2000.
[23]  P. J. Hills and M. B. Lewis, “Reducing the own-race bias in face recognition by shifting attention,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 59, no. 6, pp. 996–1002, 2006.
[24]  S. D. Goldinger, Y. He, and M. H. Papesh, “Deficits in cross-race face learning: insights from eye movements and pupillometry,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, vol. 35, no. 5, pp. 1105–1122, 2009.
[25]  J. W. Shepherd and J. B. Deregowski, “Races and faces—a comparison of the responses of Africans and Europeans to faces of the same and different races,” British Journal of Social Psychology, vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 125–133, 1981.
[26]  C. Michel, B. Rossion, J. Han, C. S. Chung, and R. Caldara, “Holistic processing is finely tuned for faces of one's own race,” Psychological Science, vol. 17, no. 7, pp. 608–615, 2006.
[27]  S. G. Young, M. J. Bernstein, and K. Hugenberg, “When do own-group biases in face recognition occur? encoding versus post-encoding,” Social Cognition, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 240–250, 2010.
[28]  J. L. Marcon, K. J. Susa, and C. A. Meissner, “Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own-versus other-race faces,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 99–103, 2009.
[29]  P. Barkowitz and J. C. Brigham, “Recognition of faces: own-race bias, incentive, and time delay,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology, vol. 18, pp. 296–301, 1982.
[30]  O. H. MacLin, M. K. MacLin, and R. S. Malpass, “Race, arousal, attention, exposure, and delay: an examination of factors moderating face recognition,” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 134–152, 2001.
[31]  A. E. Slone, J. C. Brigham, and C. A. Meissner, “Social and cognitive factors affecting the own-race bias in whites,” Basic and Applied Social Psychology, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 71–84, 2000.
[32]  E. Hirshman, “Decision processes in recognition memory: criterion shifts and the list-strength paradigm,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 302–313, 1995.
[33]  N. A. Macmillan and C. D. Creelman, Detection Theory: A User's Guide, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2nd edition, 2005.
[34]  S. E. Clark, “A re-examination of the effects of biased lineup instructions in eyewitness identification,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 395–424, 2005.
[35]  G. L. Wells, “Applied eyewitness-testimony research: system variables and estimator variables,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 36, no. 12, pp. 1546–1557, 1978.
[36]  G. L. Wells and E. A. Olson, “The other-race effect in eyewitness identification: what do we do about it?” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 230–246, 2001.
[37]  R. A. Foster, T. M. Libkuman, J. W. Schooler, and E. F. Loftus, “Consequentiality and eyewitness person identification,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 107–121, 1994.
[38]  M. Ramirez, “Multicultural/multiracial experience inventory,” in Multicultural/Multiracial Psychology: Mestizo Perspectives in Personality and Mental Health, M. Ramirez, Ed., pp. 245–254, Jason Aronson, Northvale, NJ, USA, 1998.
[39]  S. E. Clark, “Costs and benefits of eyewitness identification reform: psychological science and public policy,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 238–259, 2012.
[40]  S. D. Penrod and B. H. Bornstein, “Generalizing eyewitness reliability research,” in Memory for People, R. C. L. Lindsay, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, and M. P. Toglia, Eds., vol. 2 of Handbook of Eyewitness Psychology, pp. 529–556, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007.

Full-Text

Contact Us

[email protected]

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133