全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Eyewitness Science and the Call for Double-Blind Lineup Administration

DOI: 10.1155/2013/530523

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

For several decades, social scientists have investigated variables that can influence the accuracy of eyewitnesses’ identifications. This research has been fruitful and led to many recommendations to improve lineup procedures. Arguably, the most crucial reform social scientists advocate is double-blind lineup administration: lineups should be administered by a person who does not know the identity of the suspect. In this paper, we briefly review the classic research on expectancy effects that underlies this procedural recommendation. Then, we discuss the eyewitness research, illustrating three routes by which lineup administrators’ expectations can bias eyewitness identification evidence: effects on eyewitnesses’ identification decisions, effects on eyewitnesses’ identification confidence, and effects on administrator records of the lineup procedure. Finally, we discuss the extent to which double-blind lineup administration has been adopted among police jurisdictions in the United States and address common concerns about implementing a double-blind standard. 1. Introduction In April of 1980, 16-year-old Mario Hamilton was shot in the neck and killed in Brooklyn, New York. Thomas Charlemagne, 14, witnessed the shooting and ran to tell Mario’s 15-year-old brother, Martell. The two boys went to the police station where they were questioned separately for over six hours. Charlemagne told the police he had seen a young man named Colin Warner, 18, shooting Mario and then flees the scene in a car driven by a 15-year-old boy named Norman Simmonds. Police showed a photo lineup that contained Warner to Martell. When he did not make an identification, officers presented Warner’s photo alone and pressured Martell to identify him. Although Martell did not see the shooting first-hand, he eventually ceded to police suggestion and stated that he may have seen Warner near the scene of the crime, an identification that was used to secure Warner’s indictment and conviction in May of 1982. Warner maintained his innocence, and in 1991, Simmonds signed an affidavit indicating that he alone was responsible for the murder of Mario Hamilton. Colin Warner was eventually exonerated in 2001 after spending almost 20 years in prison for a crime he did not commit [1, 2]. Unfortunately, cases like that of Colin Warner are not uncommon. The advent of DNA testing has shed some light on the frequency with which mistaken eyewitness identifications and wrongful convictions can occur. Recent analyses indicate that mistaken eyewitness identifications are a primary cause of wrongful

References

[1]  S. R. Gross and M. Shaffer, “Exonerations in the United States, 1989–2012: A report by the National Registry of Exonerations,” 2012, http://www.law.umich.edu/special/exoneration/Documents/exonerations_us_1989_2012_full_report.pdf.
[2]  National Registry of Exonerations, “Exoneration case detail: Colin Warner,” 2012, http://www.law.umich.edu/special/exoneration/Pages/casedetail.aspx?caseid=3718.
[3]  Innocence Project, 2012, http://www.innocenceproject.org/.
[4]  G. L. Wells, A. Memon, and S. D. Penrod, “Eyewitness evidence: improving its probative value,” Psychological Science in the Public Interest, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 45–75, 2006.
[5]  G. F. Arnold, Psychology Applied to Legal Evidence and Other Constructions of Law, Thacker, Spink & Co, Calcutta, India, 1906.
[6]  A. Binet, On Suggestibility, Schleicher, Paris, France, 1900.
[7]  C. Lombroso, L’Uomo Delinquente, Hoepli, Milan, Italy, 1876.
[8]  H. Münsterberg, On the Witness Stand, Doubleday, New York, NY, USA, 1908.
[9]  L. W. Stern, “The statement as a mental achievement and product of interrogation,” Beitr?ge zur Psychologie der Assuage, vol. 3, pp. 269–415, 1904.
[10]  G. M. Whipple, “The obtaining of information: psychology of observation and report,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 15, no. 7, pp. 217–248, 1918.
[11]  G. M. Whipple, “Recent literature on the psychology of testimony,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 7, no. 11, pp. 365–368, 1910.
[12]  G. M. Whipple, “The psychology of testimony,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 8, no. 9, pp. 307–309, 1911.
[13]  G. M. Whipple, “Psychology of testimony and report,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 9, no. 7, pp. 264–269, 1912.
[14]  G. L. Wells and S. D. Penrod, “Eyewitness identification research: strengths and weaknesses of alternative methods,” in Research Methods in Forensic Psychology, B. Rosenfeld and S. D. Penrod, Eds., pp. 237–256, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011.
[15]  G. L. Wells, “Eyewitness identification: systemic reforms,” Wisconsin Law Review, vol. 2006, no. 2, pp. 615–643, 2006.
[16]  G. L. Wells, N. K. Steblay, and J. E. Dysart, “Eyewitness identification reforms: are suggestiveness-induced hits and guesses true hits?” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 7, pp. 264–271, 2012.
[17]  G. L. Wells, M. Small, S. Penrod, R. S. Malpass, S. M. Fulero, and C. A. E. Brimacombe, “Eyewitness identification procedures: recommendations for lineups and photospreads,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 22, no. 6, pp. 603–647, 1998.
[18]  S. H. Mecklenburg, Report to the Legislature of the State of Illinois: The Illinois Pilot Program on Sequential Double-Blind Identification Procedures, Illinois State Police, Springfield, Ill, USA, 2006.
[19]  D. L. Schacter, R. Dawes, L. L. Jacoby et al., “Policy forum: studying eyewitness investigations in the field,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 3–5, 2008.
[20]  G. L. Wells, “Field experiments on eyewitness identification: towards a better understanding of pitfalls and prospects,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 6–10, 2008.
[21]  G. L. Wells and D. S. Quinlivan, “Suggestive eyewitness identification procedures and the supreme court's reliability test in light of eyewitness science: 30 years later,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 1–24, 2009.
[22]  G. L. Wells, Eyewitness Identification: A System Handbook, Carswell Legal Publications, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, 1988.
[23]  R. Rosenthal, “Interpersonal expectancy effects: a 30-year perspective,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 3, pp. 176–179, 1994.
[24]  R. Rosenthal, “Covert communication in classrooms, clinics, courtrooms, and cubicles,” The American Psychologist, vol. 57, no. 11, pp. 839–849, 2002.
[25]  R. Rosenthal, “From unconscious experimenter bias to teacher expectancy effects,” in Teacher Expectancies, J. G. Dusek, V. C. Hall, and W. J. Meyer, Eds., pp. 37–65, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1985.
[26]  R. Rosenthal and K. L. Fode, “Three experiments in experimenter bias,” Psychological Reports, vol. 12, pp. 491–511, 1963.
[27]  R. Rosenthal, “Interpersonal expectations,” in Artifact in Behavioral Research, R. Rosenthal and R. L. Rosnow, Eds., pp. 181–277, Academic Press, New York, NY, USA, 1969.
[28]  R. Rosenthal and D. B. Rubin, “Interpersonal expectancy effects: the first 345 studies,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 377–415, 1978.
[29]  R. Rosenthal and K. L. Fode, “The effect of experimenter bias on the performance of the albino rat,” Behavioral Science, vol. 8, pp. 183–189, 1963.
[30]  R. Rosenthal and R. Lawson, “A longitudinal study of the effects of experimenter bias on the operant learning of laboratory rats,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 61–72, 1964.
[31]  R. Rosenthal and L. Jacobson, “Teachers' expectancies: determinants of pupils' IQ gains,” Psychological Reports, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 115–118, 1966.
[32]  R. Rosenthal and L. Jacobson, Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectations and Pupils’ Intellectual Development, Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, New York, NY, USA, 1968.
[33]  N. Ambady, D. LaPlante, T. Nguyen, N. Chaumeton, W. Levinson, and R. Rosenthal, “Surgeons' tone of voice: a clue to malpractice history,” Surgery, vol. 132, no. 1, pp. 5–9, 2002.
[34]  L. A. Learman, J. Avorn, D. E. Everitt, and R. Rosenthal, “Pygmalion in the nursing home: the effects of caregiver expectations on patient outcomes,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 38, no. 7, pp. 797–803, 1990.
[35]  T. Kida, “The impact of hypothesis-testing strategies on auditors’ use of judgment data,” Journal of Accounting Research, vol. 22, pp. 332–340, 1984.
[36]  P. D. Blanck, R. Rosenthal, A. J. Hart, and F. Bernieri, “The measure of the judge: an empirically-based framework for exploring trial judges’ behavior,” Iowa Law Review, vol. 75, pp. 653–684, 1990.
[37]  A. M. Halverson, A. J. Hart, M. Hallahan, and R. Rosenthal, “Reducing the biasing effects of judges' nonverbal behavior with simplified jury instruction,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 82, no. 4, pp. 590–598, 1997.
[38]  G. L. Wells and E. Luus, “Police lineups as experiments: social methodology as a framework for properly conducted lineups,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 16, pp. 106–117, 1990.
[39]  M. R. Phillips, B. D. McAuliff, M. B. Kovera, and B. L. Cutler, “Double-blind photoarray administration as a safeguard against investigator bias,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 84, no. 6, pp. 940–951, 1999.
[40]  R. C. L. Lindsay and G. L. Wells, “Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups. Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 70, no. 3, pp. 556–564, 1985.
[41]  A. H. Perlini and A. D. Silvaggio, “Eyewitness misidentification: single versus double-blind comparison of photospread administration,” Psychological Reports, vol. 100, no. 1, pp. 247–256, 2007.
[42]  R. M. Haw and R. P. Fisher, “Effects of administrator-witness contact on eyewitness identification accuracy,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 89, no. 6, pp. 1106–1112, 2004.
[43]  J. L. Beaudry, Video-recorded lineup procedures and detecting identification accuracy [Doctoral dissertation], Queens University, Ontario, Canada, 2008.
[44]  J. E. Dysart and L. Fugal, “Improving the sequential lineup? The effects of double blind testing and the envelope technique on post-identification feedback,” in Proceedings of the American Psychology Law Society Annual Meeting, St. Petersburg, Fla, USA, March 2006.
[45]  J. E. Dysart, A. Rainey, J. Owens, K. Chong, and V. Z. Lawson, “Double-blind lineup administration and the postidentification feedback effect,” in Proceedings of the American Psychology Law Society Annual Meeting, Jacksonville, Fla, USA, March 2008.
[46]  R. M. Haw, T. L. Mitchell, and G. L. Wells, “The influence of lineup administrator knowledge and witness perceptions on eyewitness identification decisions,” in Proceedings of the International Congress of Psychology and Law, Edinburgh, Scotland, July 2003.
[47]  M. B. Russano, J. J. Dickinson, S. A. Cass, M. B. Kovera, and B. L. Cutler, “Testing the effects of lineup administrator knowledge in simultaneous and sequential lineups,” in Proceedings of the American Psychology-Law Society Biennial Meeting, Austin, Tex, USA, March 2002.
[48]  S. M. Greathouse and M. B. Kovera, “Instruction bias and lineup presentation moderate the effects of administrator knowledge on eyewitness identification,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 70–82, 2009.
[49]  S. D. Penrod, “How well are witnesses performing?” Criminal Justice Magazine, vol. 54, pp. 36–47, 2003.
[50]  G. L. Wells and R. C. Lindsay, “On estimating the diagnosticity of eyewitness nonidentifications,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 88, no. 3, pp. 776–784, 1980.
[51]  J. L. Austin, D. M. Zimmerman, L. M. Rhead, K. A. Almeida, and M. B. Kovera, “Suspect similarity to perpetrator moderates the effect of administrator knowledge on eyewitness identification accuracy,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, San Juan, Puerto Rico, USA, March 2012.
[52]  L. M. Rhead, D. N. Rodriguez, V. Korobeynikov, J. H. Yip, and M. B. Kovera, “The effects of lineup administrator influence and mortality salience on witness identification accuracy accuracy,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, New York, NY, USA, June 2011.
[53]  D. M. Zimmerman, J. L. Austin, L. M. Rhead, K. A. Almeida, and M. B. Kovera, “Retention interval and suspect/perpetrator similarity moderate the effects of administrator knowledge on eyewitness identification,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Psychology-Law Society, San Juan, Puerto Rico, USA, March 2012.
[54]  S. E. Clark, “Costs and benefits of eyewitness identification reform: psychological science and public policy,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 7, pp. 238–259, 2012.
[55]  A. B. Douglass, C. Smith, and R. Fraser-Thill, “A problem with double-blind photospread procedures: photospread administrators use one eyewitness's confidence to influence the identification of another eyewitness,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 29, no. 5, pp. 543–562, 2005.
[56]  S. E. Clark, “Eyewitness identification reform: data, theory, and due process,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 7, pp. 279–283, 2012.
[57]  G. L. Wells, N. K. Steblay, and J. E. Dysart, A test of the simultaneous v. sequential lineup methods: an initial report of the AJS national eyewitness identification field studies, American Judicature Society, Des Moines, Iowa, USA, 2011.
[58]  A. L. Bradfield and G. L. Wells, “The perceived validity of eyewitness identification testimony: a test of the five Biggers criteria,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 24, no. 5, pp. 581–594, 2000.
[59]  Manson v. Braithwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 1977.
[60]  G. L. Wells and A. L. Bradfield, ““Good, you identified the suspect”: feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 83, no. 3, pp. 360–376, 1998.
[61]  A. L. Bradfield, G. L. Wells, and E. A. Olson, “The damaging effect of confirming feedback on the relation between eyewitness certainty and identification accuracy,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 87, no. 1, pp. 112–120, 2002.
[62]  D. N. Rodriguez and M. A. Berry, “System and estimator variables, eyewitness confidence, and the postidentification feedback effect,” American Journal of Forensic Psychology, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 17–37, 2010.
[63]  C. Semmler, N. Brewer, and G. L. Wells, “Effects of postidentification feedback on eyewitness identification and nonidentification confidence,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 89, no. 2, pp. 334–346, 2004.
[64]  A. B. Douglass and N. Steblay, “Memory distortion in eyewitnesses: a meta-analysis of the post-identification feedback effect,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 20, no. 7, pp. 859–869, 2006.
[65]  A. B. Douglass, N. Brewer, and C. Semmler, “Moderators of post-identification feedback effects on eyewitnesses' memory reports,” Legal and Criminological Psychology, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 279–292, 2010.
[66]  D. B. Wright and E. M. Skagerberg, “Postidentification feedback affects real eyewitnesses,” Psychological Science, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 172–178, 2007.
[67]  L. Garrioch and C. A. E. Brimacombe, “Lineup administrators' expectations: their impact on eyewitness confidence,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 299–315, 2001.
[68]  J. E. Dysart, V. Z. Lawson, and A. Rainer, “Blind lineup administration as a prophylactic against the postidentification feedback effect,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 312–319, 2012.
[69]  S. D. Charman and G. L. Wells, “The moderating effect of ecphoric experience on post-identification feedback: a critical test of the cues-based inference conceptualization,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 26, pp. 243–250, 2012.
[70]  N. K. Steblay, J. E. Dysart, and G. L. Wells, “Seventy-two tests of the sequential lineup superiority effect: a meta-analysis and policy discussion,” Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 99–139, 2011.
[71]  N. K. Steblay, “What we know now: the evanston Illinois field lineups,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 1–12, 2011.
[72]  R. S. Nickerson, “Confirmation bias: a ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises,” Review of General Psychology, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 175–220, 1998.
[73]  B. W. Behrman and S. L. Davey, “Eyewitness identification in actual criminal cases: an archival analysis,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 25, no. 5, pp. 475–491, 2001.
[74]  P. A. Tollestrup, J. W. Turtle, and J.C. Yuille, “Actual witnesses to robbery and fraud: an archival analysis,” in Adult Eyewitness Testimony: Current Trends and Developments, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, and M. P. Toglia, Eds., pp. 144–162, Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1994.
[75]  D. N. Rodriguez and M. A. Berry, “The influence of administrator blindness on the recording of eyewitness identification decisions,” Legal and Criminological Psychology. In press.
[76]  Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence, Eyewitness Evidence: A Guide for Law Enforcement [Booklet], United States Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Washington, DC, 1999.
[77]  Attorney General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup Identification Procedures, Department of Law and Public Safety, Office of the Attorney General, Trenton, NJ, USA, 2001.
[78]  Innocence Project, “Reevaluating lineups: Why witnesses make mistakes and how to reduce the chance of a misidentifications,” 2009, http://www.innocenceproject.org/docs/Eyewitness_ID_Report.pdf.
[79]  American Bar Association, “Statement of best practices for promoting the accuracy of eyewitness identification procedures,” 2004, http://www.americanbar.org/content/dam/aba/migrated/2011_build/death_penalty_moratorium/reduce_risk_convicting_innocent0804.authcheckdam.pdf.
[80]  International Association of Chiefs of Police, Eyewitness Identification: Model Policy, International Association of Chiefs of Police, Alexandria, Va, USA, 2010.
[81]  A. Bradfield and D. E. McQuiston, “When does evidence of eyewitness confidence inflation affect judgments in a criminal trial?” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 369–387, 2004.
[82]  A. B. Douglass and E. E. Jones, “Confidence inflation in eyewitnesses: seeing is not believing,” Legal and Criminological Psychology. In press.
[83]  J. M. Darley and P. H. Gross, “A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effects,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 44, no. 1, pp. 20–33, 1983.
[84]  J. E. Dysart, G. L. Wells, N. K. Steblay, and D. R. Mitchell, “A double-blind experiment of simultaneous versus sequential lineups using actual eyewitnesses: lab-field differences,” in Proceedings of the annual meeting of the American Psychology Law Society, San Juan, Puerto Rico, USA, March 2012.
[85]  D. N. Rodriguez and M. A. Berry, “Jurors’ causal and counterfactual evaluations of factors affecting eyewitness evidence,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Psychology Law Society, Vancouver, BC, Canada, March 2010.
[86]  D. B. Wright, M. E. Carlucci, J. R. Evans, and N. S. Compo, “Turning a blind eye to double blind line-ups,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 24, no. 6, pp. 849–867, 2010.
[87]  B. L. Cutler, S. D. Penrod, and T. E. Stuve, “Juror decision making in eyewitness identification cases,” Law and Human Behavior, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 41–55, 1988.
[88]  Model Policy and Procedure for Eyewitness Identification, State of Wisconsin Office of the Attorney General, Madison, Wis, USA.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413