全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Factor Structure of Social Cognition in Schizophrenia: Is Empathy Preserved?

DOI: 10.1155/2013/409205

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Social cognitive impairments are core features of schizophrenia and are closely associated with poor functional outcome. This study sought to identify specific aspects of social cognition and their relationships to measures of social function, quality of life, and neurocognition. Principal component analysis was performed using social cognitive measures in patients with schizophrenia and healthy matched controls and revealed three factors: Interpersonal Discomfort, Basic Social Cognition, and Empathy. Patients had higher scores on Interpersonal Discomfort and lower scores on Basic Social Cognition than controls, but the two groups were the same on Empathy. Lower social performance was significantly correlated with poor Basic Social Cognition in patients and with high Interpersonal Discomfort in controls. While neurocognition was significantly associated with Basic Social Cognition in both groups, it was not associated with Empathy. Social cognitive interventions should emphasize improving basic social cognitive processing deficits, managing Interpersonal Discomfort, and utilizing preserved capacity for empathy as a potential strength in social interactions. 1. Introduction Patients with schizophrenia have poor social cognitive skills, and these deficits greatly impact their daily functioning [1]. Additionally, social cognition has been found to mediate the relationship between neurocognition and social functioning [2, 3]. Treatment interventions have proliferated to address deficits in social cognition as ways to improve social functioning and several meta-analyses strongly support the efficacy of the interventions [4–6]. However, social cognition and social function are two broad multidimensional constructs and relatively little is known about the relationships among more specific aspects of each, either within or across the two broader spheres, or what aspects of each are most responsive to current treatments. One recent meta-analysis by Kurtz and Richardson [6] compared treatment effects on various measures of social cognition and found the greatest effects on facial affect recognition and lesser effects on measures of other aspects of social cognition. More work is needed to clarify which social cognitive constructs most directly influence social functioning [3] and which are more or less amenable to existing treatments. Understanding the social cognitive constructs that most relate to social functioning would help identify targets to treat in social cognitive interventions. Some studies have attempted to study the relationships between selected

References

[1]  S. M. Couture, D. L. Penn, and D. L. Roberts, “The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, pp. S44–S63, 2006.
[2]  M. Bell, H. W. H. Tsang, T. C. Greig, and G. J. Bryson, “Neurocognition, social cognition, perceived social discomfort, and vocational outcomes in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 738–747, 2009.
[3]  S. J. Schmidt, D. R. Mueller, and V. Roder, “Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. S41–S54, 2011.
[4]  H. C. Jin, H. K. Jin, J. Lee, and M. F. Green, “Social cognition training for individuals with schizophrenia: a review of targeted interventions,” Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 29–38, 2009.
[5]  W. P. Horan, R. S. Kern, M. F. Green, and D. L. Penn, “Social cognition training for individuals with schizophrenia: emerging evidence,” American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 205–252, 2008.
[6]  M. M. Kurtz and C. L. Richardson, “Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: a meta-analytic investigation of controlled research,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 38, no. 5, pp. 1092–1104, 2012.
[7]  R. Langdon, M. Coltheart, and P. B. Ward, “Empathetic perspective-taking is impaired in schizophrenia: evidence from a study of emotion attribution and theory of mind,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 133–155, 2006.
[8]  S. van Hooren, D. Versmissen, I. Janssen et al., “Social cognition and neurocognition as independent domains in psychosis,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 103, no. 1-3, pp. 257–265, 2008.
[9]  F. Mancuso, W. P. Horan, R. S. Kern, and M. F. Green, “Social cognition in psychosis: multidimensional structure, clinical correlates, and relationship with functional outcome,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 125, no. 2-3, pp. 143–151, 2011.
[10]  A.-K. J. Fett, W. Viechtbauer, M.-D. Dominguez, D. L. Penn, J. van Os, and L. Krabbendam, “The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 573–588, 2011.
[11]  U. M. Mehta, J. Thirthalli, D. K. Subbakrishna, et al., “Social and neuro-cognition as distinct cognitive factors in schizophrenia: a systematic review,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 148, no. 1–3, pp. 3–11, 2013.
[12]  A. E. Pinkham, D. L. Penn, M. F. Green, et al., “The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: results of the expert survey and RAND panel,” Schizophrenia Bulletin. In press.
[13]  M. J. Smith, W. P. Horan, T. M. Karpouzian, et al., “Self-reported empathy deficits are uniquely associated with poor functioning in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 137, no. 1, pp. 196–202, 2012.
[14]  R. E. Gur, C. McGrath, R. M. Chan et al., “An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in patients with schizophrenia,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 159, no. 12, pp. 1992–1999, 2002.
[15]  B. I. Turetsky, C. G. Kohler, T. Indersmitten, M. T. Bhati, D. Charbonnier, and R. C. Gur, “Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia:When and why does it go awry?” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 94, no. 1–3, pp. 253–263, 2007.
[16]  C. G. Kohler, J. B. Walker, E. A. Martin, K. M. Healey, and P. J. Moberg, “Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 36, no. 5, pp. 1009–1019, 2010.
[17]  M. F. Green and J. Lee, “Neural bases of emotional experience versus perception in schizophrenia,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 71, no. 2, pp. 96–97, 2012.
[18]  S. F. Taylor, J. Kang, I. S. Brege, I. F. Tso, A. Hosanagar, and T. D. Johnson, “Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of emotion perception and experience in schizophrenia,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 71, no. 2, pp. 136–145, 2012.
[19]  A. M. Kring and E. K. Moran, “Emotional response deficits in schizophrenia: insights from affective science,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 34, no. 5, pp. 819–834, 2008.
[20]  A. S. Cohen and K. S. Minor, “Emotional experience in patients with schizophrenia revisited: meta-analysis of laboratory studies,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 36, no. 1, pp. 143–150, 2010.
[21]  S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, J. Aharon-Peretz, and D. Perry, “Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions,” Brain, vol. 132, no. 3, pp. 617–627, 2009.
[22]  M. Bell, Bell Object Relations and Reality Testing Inventory (BORRTI) Manual, Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, Calif, USA, 1995.
[23]  M. D. Bell, T. C. Greig, G. Bryson, and E. Kaplan, “Patterns of object relations and reality testing deficits in schizophrenia: clusters and their symptom and personality correlates,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 57, no. 12, pp. 1353–1367, 2001.
[24]  APA, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA, 4th edition, 1994.
[25]  M. B. First, et al., Structured Clinical Interview For DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. (SCID-I/P), Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA, 1998.
[26]  M. B. First, et al., Structured Clinical Interview For DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition. (SCID-I/NP), Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA, 1998.
[27]  N. C. Andreasen, M. Pressler, P. Nopoulos, D. Miller, and B.-C. Ho, “Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 67, no. 3, pp. 255–262, 2010.
[28]  S. R. Kay, A. Fiszbein, and L. A. Opler, “The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 261–276, 1987.
[29]  K. H. Nuechterlein and M. F. Green, MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, MATRICS Assessment, Los Angeles, Calif, USA, 2006.
[30]  K. H. Nuechterlein, D. M. Barch, J. M. Gold, T. E. Goldberg, M. F. Green, and R. K. Heaton, “Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 72, no. 1, pp. 29–39, 2004.
[31]  A. Klin, “Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning Autism and Asperger syndrome: the social attribution task,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 41, no. 7, pp. 831–846, 2000.
[32]  A. Klin and W. Jones, “Attributing social and physical meaning to ambiguous visual displays in individuals with higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 40–53, 2006.
[33]  M. D. Bell, J. M. Fiszdon, T. C. Greig, and B. E. Wexler, “Social attribution test - multiple choice (SAT-MC) in schizophrenia: comparison with community sample and relationship to neurocognitive, social cognitive and symptom measures,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 122, no. 1–3, pp. 164–171, 2010.
[34]  F. Heider and M. Simmel, “An experimental study of apparent behavior,” American Journal of Psychology, vol. 57, no. 2, pp. 243–259, 1944.
[35]  M. Bell, G. Bryson, and P. Lysaker, “Positive and negative affect recognition in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control subjects,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 73, no. 1-2, pp. 73–82, 1997.
[36]  R. Corcoran, G. Mercer, and C. D. Frith, “Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: Investigating 'theory of mind' in people with schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 5–13, 1995.
[37]  T. C. Greig, G. J. Bryson, and M. D. Bell, “Theory of mind performance in schizophrenia: diagnostic, symptom, and neuropsychological correlates,” Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, vol. 192, no. 1, pp. 12–18, 2004.
[38]  M. D. Bell, Manual For the Bell Object Relations Reality Testing Inventory, Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, Calif, USA, 1995.
[39]  S. Li and M. D. Bell, “BORRTI journal articles and annotated bibliography,” 2008, http://portal.wpspublish.com/portal/page?_pageid=53, 70155&_dad=portal&_schema=PORTAL.
[40]  M. H. Davis, “Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 44, no. 1, pp. 113–126, 1983.
[41]  T. L. Patterson, S. Moscona, C. L. McKibbin, et al., “Social skills performance assessment among older patients with schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 48, no. 2, pp. 351–360, 2001.
[42]  J. D. Mayer, P. Salovey, and D. R. Caruso, Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), Multi-Health Systems, Toronto, Canada, 2002.
[43]  D. W. Heinrichs, T. E. Hanlon, and W. T. Carpenter Jr., “The Quality of Life Scale: an instrument for rating the schizophrenic deficit syndrome,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 388–398, 1984.
[44]  R. B. Cattell, “The scree test for the number of factors,” Multivariate Behavioral Research, vol. 1, no. 2, pp. 245–276, 1966.
[45]  A. B. Costello and J. W. Osborne, “Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis,” Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, vol. 10, no. 7, pp. 1–9, 2005.
[46]  R. L. E. P. Reniers, R. Corcoran, R. Drake, N. M. Shryane, and B. A. V?llm, “The QCAE: a questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy,” Journal of Personality Assessment, vol. 93, no. 1, pp. 84–95, 2011.
[47]  M. D. Bell, S. Corbera, J. K. Johannesen, et al., “Social cognitive impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia: are there subtypes with distinct functional correlates?” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 186–196, 2013.
[48]  S. M. Couture, E. L. Granholm, and S. C. Fish, “A path model investigation of neurocognition, theory of mind, social competence, negative symptoms and real-world functioning in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 125, no. 2-3, pp. 152–160, 2011.
[49]  E. Brunet-Gouet, A. M. Achim, D. Vistoli, C. Passerieux, M.-C. Hardy-Baylé, and P. L. Jackson, “The study of social cognition with neuroimaging methods as a means to explore future directions of deficit evaluation in schizophrenia?” Psychiatry Research, vol. 190, no. 1, pp. 23–31, 2011.
[50]  A. M. Achim, R. Ouellet, M.-A. Roy, and P. L. Jackson, “Assessment of empathy in first-episode psychosis and meta-analytic comparison with previous studies in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 190, no. 1, pp. 3–8, 2011.
[51]  A. Sparks, S. McDonald, B. Lino, et al., “Social cognition, empathy and functional outcome in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 122, no. 1, Article ID 206095, pp. 172–178, 2010.
[52]  G. N. Savla, L. Vella, C. C. Armstrong, et al., “Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 39, no. 5, pp. 979–992, 2012.
[53]  M. F. Green, G. Hellemann, W. P. Horan, et al., “From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 69, no. 12, pp. 1216–1224, 2012.
[54]  R. W. Morris, C. S. Weickert, and C. M. Loughland, “Emotional face processing in schizophrenia,” Current Opinion in Psychiatry, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 140–146, 2009.
[55]  T. S. Woodward, R. Mizrahi, M. Menon, and B. K. Christensen, “Correspondences between theory of mind, jumping to conclusions, neuropsychological measures and the symptoms of schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 170, no. 2-3, pp. 119–123, 2009.
[56]  H. Haker and W. R?ssler, “Empathy in schizophrenia: impaired resonance,” European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 259, no. 6, pp. 352–361, 2009.
[57]  C. Montag, A. Heinz, D. Kunz, and J. Gallinat, “Self-reported empathic abilities in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 92, no. 1-3, pp. 85–89, 2007.
[58]  S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, S. Shur, L. Barcai-Goodman, S. Medlovich, H. Harari, and Y. Levkovitz, “Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 149, no. 1–3, pp. 11–23, 2007.
[59]  S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, S. Shur, H. Harari, and Y. Levkovitz, “Neurocognitive basis of impaired empathy in schizophrenia,” Neuropsychology, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 431–438, 2007.
[60]  B. Derntl, A. Finkelmeyer, T. K. Toygar et al., “Generalized deficit in all core components of empathy in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 108, no. 1–3, pp. 197–206, 2009.
[61]  D. R. Combs, D. L. Penn, M. Wicher, and E. Waldheter, “The Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ): a new measure for evaluating hostile social-cognitive biases in paranoia,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 128–143, 2007.
[62]  D. Jolliffe and D. P. Farrington, “Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale,” Journal of Adolescence, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 589–611, 2006.

Full-Text

Contact Us

[email protected]

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133