全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
Scientifica  2014 

Brain Mechanisms and Reading Remediation: More Questions Than Answers

DOI: 10.1155/2014/802741

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Dyslexia is generally diagnosed in childhood and is characterised by poor literacy skills with associated phonological and perceptual problems. Compensated dyslexic readers are adult readers who have a documented history of childhood dyslexia but as adults can read and comprehend written text well. Uncompensated dyslexic readers are adults who similarly have a documented history of reading impairment but remain functionally reading-impaired all their lives. There is little understanding of the neurophysiological basis for how or why some children become compensated, while others do not, and there is little knowledge about neurophysiological changes that occur with remedial programs for reading disability. This paper will review research looking at reading remediation, particularly in the context of the underlying neurophysiology. 1. Brain Mechanisms and Reading Remediation: More Questions Than Answers Approximately 10% of children suffer a specific reading difficulty such as dyslexia [1]. Despite some residual deficits in core skills, (e.g., phonological processing), some of these individuals will ultimately learn good reading skills as adults (become compensated), while others will remain functionally reading-impaired all their lives (uncompensated) [2, 3]. On the last page of her seminal book on dyslexia, Snowling [2] concludes “The research agenda for the next decade must certainly be directed to the treatment resisters,… those poor readers who do not respond well to current intervention programs.” Yet despite the huge personal and social costs of dyslexia, virtually nothing is known about how or why some young dyslexic readers ultimately learn to read, while others remain functionally dyslexic their whole lives. The aim of the current review is to consider some of the research on reading remediation, particularly within the context of underlying brain mechanisms. A number of reviews have been conducted regarding the functional organisation of the normal reading network in the brain (refer to [4] for a recent review) and there is some research that has looked at compensatory brain mechanisms that develop as poor readers develop good reading skills [5]. However, a full understanding of how cortical networks develop in response to acquiring reading skills requires not only an understanding of what those networks look like but also an understanding of how those networks are functionally connected. Functional connectivity in language is well documented (e.g., [6]), and it is common for researchers to draw on this literature to also describe reading

References

[1]  Boon, Submission to the Legislative Council of NSW Standing Committee on Social Issues, 2001.
[2]  M. Snowling, Dyslexia, Malden and Blackwell, 2nd edition, 2000.
[3]  M. Bruck, “Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 439–454, 1990.
[4]  C. Price, “A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading,” Neuroimage, vol. 62, pp. 816–847, 2012.
[5]  M. Koyama, A. Di Martino, C. Kelly et al., “Cortical signatures of Dyslexia and remediation: an intrinsic functional connectivity approach,” PLOS ONE, vol. 8, no. 2, article e55454, 14 pages, 2013.
[6]  F. Pulvermüller, “A brain perspective on language mechanisms: from discrete neuronal ensembles to serial order,” Progress in Neurobiology, vol. 67, no. 2, pp. 85–111, 2002.
[7]  J. Hatcher, M. J. Snowling, and Y. M. Griffiths, “Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education,” British Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 72, no. 1, pp. 119–133, 2002.
[8]  S.-A. Lindgrén and M. Laine, “Cognitive-linguistic performances of multilingual university students suspected of dyslexia,” Dyslexia, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 184–200, 2011.
[9]  N. E. Jackson and H. L. Doellinger, “Resilient readers? University students who are poor recoders but sometimes good text comprehenders,” Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 94, no. 1, pp. 64–78, 2002.
[10]  M. Bruck, “Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 28, no. 5, pp. 874–886, 1992.
[11]  R. P. Fink, “Literacy development in successful men and women with Dyslexia,” Annals of Dyslexia, vol. 48, pp. 311–342, 1998.
[12]  A. M. Gallagher, V. Laxon, E. Armstrong, and U. Frith, “Phonological difficulties in high-functioning dyslexics,” Reading and Writing, vol. 8, no. 6, pp. 499–509, 1996.
[13]  J. R. Nelson, G. J. Benner, and J. Gonzalez, “Learner characteristics that influence the treatment effectiveness of early literacy interventions: a meta-analytic review,” Learning Disabilities Research Practice, vol. 18, pp. 255–267, 2003.
[14]  S. H. Greenblatt, “Left occipital lobectomy and the preangular anatomy of reading,” Brain and Language, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 576–595, 1990.
[15]  S. H. Greenblatt, “Alexia without agraphia or hemianopsia,” Brain, vol. 96, no. 2, pp. 307–316, 1973.
[16]  C. Rosazza, I. Appollonio, V. Isella, and T. Shallice, “Qualitatively different forms of pure alexia,” Cognitive Neuropsychology, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 393–418, 2007.
[17]  K. Patterson and J. Kay, “Letter-by-letter reading: psychological descriptions of a neurological syndrome,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 411–441, 1982.
[18]  M. A. Tamhankar, H. B. Coslett, M. J. Fisher, L. N. Sutton, and G. T. Liu, “Alexia without agraphia following biopsy of a left thalamic tumor,” Pediatric Neurology, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 140–142, 2004.
[19]  S. Erdem and T. Kansu, “Alexia without either agraphia or hemianopia in temporal lobe lesion due to herpes simplex encephalitis,” Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 102–104, 1995.
[20]  V. W. Henderson, R. B. Friedman, E. L. Teng, and J. M. Weiner, “Left hemisphere pathways in reading: inferences from pure alexia without hemianopia,” Neurology, vol. 35, no. 7, pp. 962–968, 1985.
[21]  Y. Sakurai, S. Takeuchi, T. Takada, E. Horiuchi, H. Nakase, and M. Sakuta, “Alexia caused by a fusiform or posterior inferior temporal lesion,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences, vol. 178, no. 1, pp. 42–51, 2000.
[22]  J. Déjerine, “Sur un cas de cécité verbale avec agraphie, suivi d’autopsie,” Comptes Rendus Hebdomadaires des Séances et Mémoires de la Société de Biologie, vol. 3, pp. 197–201, 1891.
[23]  C. Chase and A. R. Jenner, “Magnocellular visual deficits affect temporal processing of dyslexics,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 682, pp. 326–329, 1993.
[24]  P. Cornelissen, A. Richardson, A. Mason, S. Fowler, and J. Stein, “Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls,” Vision Research, vol. 35, no. 10, pp. 1483–1494, 1995.
[25]  G. F. Eden, J. W. VanMeter, J. M. Rumsey, J. M. Maisog, R. P. Woods, and T. A. Zeffiro, “Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging,” Nature, vol. 382, no. 6586, pp. 66–69, 1996.
[26]  P. C. Hansen, J. F. Stein, S. R. Orde, J. L. Winter, and J. B. Talcott, “Are dyslexics' visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function?” NeuroReport, vol. 12, no. 7, pp. 1527–1530, 2001.
[27]  M. S. Livingstone, G. D. Rosen, F. W. Drislane, and A. M. Galaburda, “Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 88, no. 18, pp. 7943–7947, 1991.
[28]  S. Lehmkuhle, R. P. Garzia, L. Turner, T. Hash, and J. A. Baro, “A defective visual pathway in children with reading disability,” New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 328, no. 14, pp. 989–996, 1993.
[29]  K. Pammer and C. Wheatley, “Isolating the M(y)-cell response in dyslexia using the spatial frequency doubling illusion,” Vision Research, vol. 41, no. 16, pp. 2139–2147, 2001.
[30]  K. Pammer, R. Lavis, P. Hansen, and P. L. Cornelissen, “Symbol-string sensitivity and children's reading,” Brain and Language, vol. 89, no. 3, pp. 601–610, 2004.
[31]  K. Pammer, R. Lavis, and P. Cornelissen, “Visual encoding mechanisms and their relationship to text presentation preference,” Dyslexia, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 77–94, 2004.
[32]  K. Pammer, R. Lavis, C. Cooper, P. C. Hansen, and P. L. Cornelissen, “Symbol-string sensitivity and adult performance in lexical decision,” Brain and Language, vol. 94, no. 3, pp. 278–296, 2005.
[33]  K. Pammer and A. Kevan, “The contribution of visual sensitivity, phonological processing, and nonverbal IQ to children's reading,” Scientific Studies of Reading, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 33–53, 2007.
[34]  U. Goswami, J. Thomson, U. Richardson et al., “Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: a new hypothesis,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 99, no. 16, pp. 10911–10916, 2002.
[35]  A. M. Galaburda, “Neurology of developmental dyslexia,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 237–242, 1993.
[36]  S. Heim, C. Eulitz, J. Kaufmann et al., “Atypical organisation of the auditory cortex in dyslexia as revealed by MEG,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 38, no. 13, pp. 1749–1759, 2000.
[37]  K. I. McAnally and J. F. Stein, “Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia,” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, vol. 263, no. 1373, pp. 961–965, 1996.
[38]  S. Birch and C. Chase, “Visual and language processing deficits in compensated and uncompensated college students with dyslexia,” Journal of Learning Disabilities, vol. 37, no. 5, pp. 389–410, 2004.
[39]  J. H?m?l?inen, P. Leppanen, M. Torppa, K. Muller, and H. Lyytinen, “Detection of sound rise time by adults with dyslexia,” Brain and Language, vol. 94, pp. 32–42, 2005.
[40]  D. Flowers, Developmental and Acquired Dyslexia, Kluwer Academic, Dodrecht, The Netherlands, 1995.
[41]  M. J. Snowling, “The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 294–305, 1980.
[42]  E. Corcos and D. Willows, Visual Processes in Reading and Reading Disabilities, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1993.
[43]  E. Paulesu, U. Frith, M. Snowling et al., “Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning,” Brain, vol. 119, no. 1, pp. 143–157, 1996.
[44]  S. E. Shaywitz, B. A. Shaywitz, R. K. Fulbright et al., “Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 54, no. 1, pp. 25–33, 2003.
[45]  M. Ingvar, P. Af Trampe, T. Greitz, L. Eriksson, S. Stone-Elander, and C. von Euler, “Residual differences in language processing in compensated dyslexics revealed in simple word reading tasks,” Brain and Language, vol. 83, no. 2, pp. 249–267, 2002.
[46]  K. Cain and J. Oakhill, “Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties,” British Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 76, no. 4, pp. 683–696, 2006.
[47]  K. R. Pugh, W. E. Mencl, A. R. Jenner et al., “Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability,” Journal of Communication Disorders, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 479–492, 2001.
[48]  K. Nation and M. J. Snowling, “Individual differences in contextual facilitation: evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension,” Child Development, vol. 69, no. 4, pp. 996–1011, 1998.
[49]  K. Nation and M. J. Snowling, “Semantic processing and the development of word-recognition skills: evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties,” Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 85–101, 1998.
[50]  S. Bookheimer, “Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 25, pp. 151–188, 2002.
[51]  N. Brunswick, E. McCrory, C. J. Price, C. D. Frith, and U. Frith, “Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics. A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz?” Brain, vol. 122, no. 10, pp. 1901–1917, 1999.
[52]  J. M. Rumsey, K. Nace, B. Donohue, D. Wise, J. M. Maisog, and P. Andreason, “A positron emission tomographic study of impaired word recognition and phonological processing in dyslexic men,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 54, no. 5, pp. 562–573, 1997.
[53]  K. Stanovich, “Toward an interactive compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency,” Reading Research Quarterly, vol. 16, pp. 32–71, 1980.
[54]  J. Kujala, K. Pammer, P. Cornelissen, A. Roebroeck, E. Formisano, and R. Salmelin, “Phase coupling in a cerebro-cerebellar network at 8–13 Hz during reading,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 17, no. 6, pp. 1476–1485, 2007.
[55]  B. A. Wright and S. G. Zecker, “Learning problems, delayed development, and puberty,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 101, no. 26, pp. 9942–9946, 2004.
[56]  M. S. Brainard and E. I. Knudsen, “Sensitive periods for visual calibration of the auditory space map in the barn owl optic tectum,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 18, no. 10, pp. 3929–3942, 1998.
[57]  D. V. M. Bishop and G. M. McArthur, “Immature cortical responses to auditory stimuli in specific language impairment: evidence from ERPs to rapid tone sequences,” Developmental Science, vol. 7, no. 4, pp. F11–F18, 2004.
[58]  D. V. M. Bishop and G. M. McArthur, “Individual differences in auditory processing in specific language impairment: a follow-up study using event-related potentials and behavioural thresholds,” Cortex, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 327–341, 2005.
[59]  G. McArthur and D. Bishop, “Event-related potentials reflect individual differences in age-invariant auditory skills,” NeuroReport, vol. 13, no. 8, pp. 1079–1082, 2002.
[60]  G. M. McArthur and D. V. M. Bishop, “Speech and non-speech processing in people with specific language impairment: a behavioural and electrophysiological study,” Brain and Language, vol. 94, no. 3, pp. 260–273, 2005.
[61]  R. Salmelin, E. Service, P. Kiesil?, K. Uutela, and O. Salonen, “Impaired visual word processing in dyslexia revealed with magnetoencephalography,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 40, no. 2, pp. 157–162, 1996.
[62]  D. L. Flowers, F. B. Wood, and C. E. Naylor, “Regional cerebral blood flow correlates of language processes in reading disability,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 48, no. 6, pp. 637–643, 1991.
[63]  K. Pugh, W. Mencl, A. Jenner, L. Katz, S. Frost, et al., “Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia),” Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, vol. 6, pp. 207–213, 2000.
[64]  S. E. Shaywitz, B. A. Shaywitz, K. R. Pugh et al., “Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 95, no. 5, pp. 2636–2641, 1998.
[65]  T. Klingberg, M. Hedehus, E. Temple et al., “Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging,” Neuron, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 493–500, 2000.
[66]  B. Horwitz, J. M. Rumsey, and B. C. Donohue, “Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 95, no. 15, pp. 8939–8944, 1998.
[67]  H. Wimmer, M. Schurz, D. Sturm et al., “A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: an fMRI study,” Cortex, vol. 46, no. 10, pp. 1284–1298, 2010.
[68]  F. Hoeft, B. D. McCandliss, J. M. Black et al., “Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 108, no. 1, pp. 361–366, 2011.
[69]  E. H. Aylward, T. L. Richards, V. W. Berninger et al., “Instructional treatment associated with changes in brain activation in children with dyslexia,” Neurology, vol. 61, no. 2, pp. 212–219, 2003.
[70]  G. F. Eden, K. M. Jones, K. Cappell et al., “Neural changes following remediation in adult developmental dyslexia,” Neuron, vol. 44, no. 3, pp. 411–422, 2004.
[71]  A. Meyler, T. A. Keller, V. L. Cherkassky, J. D. E. Gabrieli, and M. A. Just, “Modifying the brain activation of poor readers during sentence comprehension with extended remedial instruction: a longitudinal study of neuroplasticity,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 46, no. 10, pp. 2580–2592, 2008.
[72]  E. Temple, G. K. Deutsch, R. A. Poldrack et al., “Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: evidence from functional MRI,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 100, no. 5, pp. 2860–2865, 2003.
[73]  T. A. Keller and M. A. Just, “Altering cortical connectivity: remediation-induced changes in the white matter of poor readers,” Neuron, vol. 64, no. 5, pp. 624–631, 2009.
[74]  A. J. Krafnick, D. L. Flowers, E. M. Napoliello, and G. F. Eden, “Gray matter volume changes following reading intervention in dyslexic children,” NeuroImage, vol. 57, no. 3, pp. 733–741, 2011.
[75]  B. Biswal, F. Z. Yetkin, V. M. Haughton, and J. S. Hyde, “Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI,” Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 537–541, 1995.
[76]  R. Buckner, F. Krienen, and T. Yeo, “Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 16, pp. 832–837, 2013.
[77]  K. Pammer, P. C. Hansen, M. L. Kringelbach et al., “Visual word recognition: the first half second,” NeuroImage, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 1819–1825, 2004.
[78]  L. A. Hald, M. C. M. Bastiaansen, and P. Hagoort, “EEG theta and gamma responses to semantic violations in online sentence processing,” Brain and Language, vol. 96, no. 1, pp. 90–105, 2006.
[79]  F. Varela, J.-P. Lachaux, E. Rodriguez, and J. Martinerie, “The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 229–239, 2001.
[80]  W. Singer, “Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations?” Neuron, vol. 24, no. 1, pp. 49–65, 1999.
[81]  G. Thut, C. Miniussi, and J. Gross, “The functional importance of rhythmic activity in the brain,” Current Biology, vol. 22, pp. 658–663, 2012.
[82]  J. Burke, K. Zaghloul, J. Jacobs et al., “Synchronous and asynchronous theta and gamma activity during episodic memory formation,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 33, pp. 292–304, 2013.
[83]  B. C. Lega, J. Jacobs, and M. Kahana, “Human hippocampal theta oscillations and the formation of episodic memories,” Hippocampus, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 748–761, 2012.
[84]  R. Moran, P. Campo, F. Maestu, R. Reilly, R. Dolan, and B. Strange, “Peak frequency in the theta and alpha bands correlates with human working memory capacity,” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 4, pp. 1–12, 2010.
[85]  P. Sauseng, W. Klimesch, K. F. Heise et al., “Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity,” Current Biology, vol. 19, no. 21, pp. 1846–1852, 2009.
[86]  O. Jensen, J. Kaiser, and J.-P. Lachaux, “Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 30, no. 7, pp. 317–324, 2007.
[87]  Z. Gao, A. Goldstein, Y. Harpaz, M. Hansel, E. Zion-Golumbic, and S. Bentin, “A magnetoencephalographic study of face processing: M170, gamma-band oscillations and source localization,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 34, no. 8, pp. 1783–1795, 2013.
[88]  U. Friese, G. G. Supp, J. F. Hipp, A. K. Engel, and T. Gruber, “Oscillatory MEG gamma band activity dissociates perceptual and conceptual aspects of visual object processing: a combined repetition/conceptual priming study,” NeuroImage, vol. 59, no. 1, pp. 861–871, 2012.
[89]  J. Martinovic, T. Gruber, and M. M. Müller, “Coding of visual object features and feature conjunctions in the human brain,” PLoS ONE, vol. 3, no. 11, Article ID e3781, 2008.
[90]  C. Tallon-Baudry and O. Bertrand, “Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 3, no. 4, pp. 151–162, 1999.
[91]  M. Nazari, E. Mosanezhad, T. Hashemi, and A. Jahan, “The effectiveness of neurofeedback training on EEG coherence and neuropsychological functions in children with reading disability,” Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, vol. 43, pp. 315–322, 2012.
[92]  G. M. McArthur, J. H. Hogben, V. T. Edwards, S. M. Heath, and E. D. Mengler, “On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 41, no. 7, pp. 869–874, 2000.
[93]  S. Heim, A. Keil, N. Choundry, J. Friedman, and A. Benasich, “Early gamma oscillations during rapid auditory processing in children with a language-learning impairment: changes in neural mass activity after training,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 51, pp. 990–1001, 2013.
[94]  M. Koyama, A. DiMartino, A. Kelly, et al., “Cortical signatures of dyslexia and remediation: an intrinsic functional connectivity approach,” PLoS ONE, vol. 8, pp. 1–14, 2013.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133