全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Trait Hostility, Perceived Stress, and Sleep Quality in a Sample of Normal Sleepers

DOI: 10.1155/2013/735812

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Objective. To date, no studies have directly examined the effects of cognitive trait hostility on prospectively assessed sleep quality. This is important as individuals with heightened trait hostility demonstrate similar patterns of reactivity to perceived stressors as is often reported by poor sleepers. The present study hypothesized that increased trait hostility is associated with poorer subjective sleep quality and that perceived stress mediates this relationship. Methods. A sample of 66 normal sleepers completed daily sleep and stress logs for two weeks. Trait hostility was measured retrospectively. Results. The cognitive dimension of trait hostility was significantly correlated with subjectively rated sleep quality indicators, and these relationships were significantly mediated by perceived daily stress. Individuals with higher levels of trait cognitive hostility reported increased levels of perceived stress which accounted for their poorer sleep ratings as measured by both retrospective and prospective measures. Conclusions. Overall, the findings indicate that high levels of cognitive hostility are a significant risk factor for disturbed sleep and suggest that this might be a fruitful target for clinical intervention. 1. Introduction The relationship between trait hostility and sleep quality remains underexplored despite the empirical indications that individuals with increased hostility experience more stress, a factor known to degrade sleep quality [1–4]. Despite its widespread use as an outcome variable, there is no standard definition for sleep quality. Investigators frequently use both objective measures, such as total sleep duration, efficiency, and sleep onset latency, as well as subjective self-report measures to assess sleep quality [5]. While a number of variables can influence one’s day-to-day sleep outcomes, stress is strongly associated with sleep disruption [1, 6]. Stress is thought to act on sleep primarily via increased cognitive and somatic arousal during the presleep period [7]. In a recent study, Morin et al. [6] confirmed that hyperarousal during the presleep period mediates the relationship between stress and sleep quality in normal and disordered sleepers alike. The study suggests that individual variation in stress reactivity determines the extent to which stress degrades sleep quality. Consistent with this finding, studies have demonstrated that poor sleepers are characteristically hyperreactive to stress [1, 2, 6, 8]. Pronounced stress reactions are characteristic of individuals who score highly on measures of trait

References

[1]  S. Rubman, J. Brantley, W. Waters, G. Jones, J. Constans, and C. Findley, “Daily stress and insomnia,” in Proceedings of the Meeting of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, Chicago, Ill, USA, 1990.
[2]  A. Sadeh, G. Keinan, and K. Daon, “Effects of stress on sleep: the moderating role of coping style,” Health Psychology, vol. 23, no. 5, pp. 542–545, 2004.
[3]  E. C. Suarez and R. B. Williams, “Situational determinants of cardiovascular and emotional reactivity in high and low hostile men,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 51, no. 4, pp. 404–418, 1989.
[4]  T. Q. Miller, T. W. Smith, C. W. Turner, M. L. Guijarro, and A. J. Hallet, “A meta-analytic review of research on hostility and physical health,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 119, no. 2, pp. 322–348, 1996.
[5]  A. D. Krystal and J. D. Edinger, “Measuring sleep quality,” Sleep Medicine, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. S10–S17, 2008.
[6]  C. M. Morin, S. Rodrigue, and H. Ivers, “Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 65, no. 2, pp. 259–267, 2003.
[7]  A. Harvey, “A cognitive model of insomnia,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 40, no. 8, pp. 869–893, 2002.
[8]  A. J. Guastella and M. L. Moulds, “The impact of rumination on sleep quality following a stressful life event,” Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 42, no. 6, pp. 1151–1162, 2007.
[9]  J. C. Barefoot, K. A. Dodge, B. L. Peterson, W. G. Dahlstrom, and R. B. Williams, “The Cook-Medley hostility scale: item content and ability to predict survival,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 51, no. 1, pp. 46–57, 1989.
[10]  L. D. Jamner, D. Shapiro, I. B. Goldstein, and R. Hug, “Ambulatory blood pressure and heart rate in paramedics: effects of cynical hostility and defensiveness,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 53, no. 4, pp. 393–406, 1991.
[11]  E. Brondolo, K. I. Grantham, W. Karlin et al., “Trait hostility and ambulatory blood pressure among traffic enforcement agents: the effects of stressful social interactions,” Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 110–121, 2009.
[12]  N. Gran?, J. Vahtera, M. Virtanen, L. Keltikangas-J?rvinen, and M. Kivim?ki, “Association of hostility with sleep duration and sleep disturbances in an employee population,” International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 73–80, 2008.
[13]  J. L. Ireland and V. Culpin, “The relationship between sleeping problems and aggression, anger, and impulsivity in a population of juvenile and young offenders,” Journal of Adolescent Health, vol. 38, no. 6, pp. 649–655, 2006.
[14]  I. Brissette and S. Cohen, “The contribution of individual differences in hostility to the associations between daily interpersonal conflict, affect, and sleep,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 28, no. 9, pp. 1265–1274, 2002.
[15]  H. Ursin and H. R. Eriksen, “The cognitive activation theory of stress,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 29, no. 5, pp. 567–592, 2004.
[16]  V. J. Fortunato and J. Harsh, “Stress and sleep quality: the moderating role of negative affectivity,” Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 41, no. 5, pp. 825–836, 2006.
[17]  S. Brand, M. Gerber, U. Pühse, and E. Holsboer-Trachsler, “Depression, hypomania, and dysfunctional sleep-related cognitions as mediators between stress and insomnia: the best advice is not always found on the pillow,” International Journal of Stress Management, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 114–134, 2010.
[18]  A. N. Vgontzas, E. O. Bixler, H. M. Lin et al., “Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: clinical implications,” Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, vol. 86, no. 8, pp. 3787–3794, 2001.
[19]  M. H. Burleson, K. M. Poehlmann, L. C. Hawkley et al., “Neuroendocrine and cardiovascular reactivity to stress in mid-aged and older women: long-term temporal consistency of individual differences,” Psychophysiology, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 358–369, 2003.
[20]  J. F. Brosschot, S. Pieper, and J. F. Thayer, “Expanding stress theory: prolonged activation and perseverative cognition,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 30, no. 10, pp. 1043–1049, 2005.
[21]  J. F. Brosschot and J. F. Thayer, “Anger inhibition, cardiovascular recovery, and vagal function: a model of the link between hostility and cardiovascular disease,” Annals of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 326–332, 1998.
[22]  M. F. Scheier and M. W. Bridges, “Person variables and health: personality predispositions and acute psychological states as shared determinants for disease,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 57, no. 3, pp. 255–268, 1995.
[23]  R. Williams, J. Barefoot, and R. Shekell, “The health consequences of hostility,” in Anger and Hostility in Cardiovascular Disease and Behavioral Disorders, M. Chesney and R. Rosenman, Eds., pp. 173–185, Hemisphere, Columbia, SC, USA, 1985.
[24]  S. A. Neumann, S. R. Waldstein, J. J. Sollers, J. F. Thayer, and J. D. Sorkin, “Hostility and distraction have differential influences on cardiovascular recovery from anger recall in women,” Health Psychology, vol. 23, no. 6, pp. 631–640, 2004.
[25]  H. A. Demaree and D. E. Everhart, “Healthy high-hostiles: reduced parasympathetic activity and decreased sympathovagal flexibility during negative emotional processing,” Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 457–469, 2004.
[26]  B. L. Fredrickson, K. E. Maynard, M. J. Helms, T. L. Haney, I. C. Siegler, and J. C. Barefoot, “Hostility predicts magnitude and duration of blood pressure response to anger,” Journal of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 229–243, 2000.
[27]  R. D. Chervin, J. E. Dillon, K. H. Archbold, and D. L. Ruzicka, “Conduct problems and symptoms of sleep disorders in children,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 201–208, 2003.
[28]  P. L. Haynes, R. R. Bootzin, L. Smith, J. Cousins, M. Cameron, and S. Stevens, “Sleep and aggression in substance-abusing adolescents: results from an integrative behavioral sleep-treatment pilot program,” Sleep, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 512–520, 2006.
[29]  W. W. Cook and D. M. Medley, “Proposed hostility and Pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 38, no. 6, pp. 414–418, 1954.
[30]  D. J. Buysse, C. F. Reynolds, T. H. Monk, S. R. Berman, and D. J. Kupfer, “The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 193–213, 1989.
[31]  S. Cohen, T. Kamarck, and R. Mermelstein, “A global measure of perceived stress,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 385–396, 1983.
[32]  S. Cohen, “Contrasting the Hassles scale and the perceived stress scale. Who's really measuring appraised stress?” American Psychologist, vol. 41, no. 6, pp. 716–718, 1986.
[33]  K. Han, N. Weed, R. Calhoun, and J. Butcher, “Psychometric characteristics of the MMPI-2 cook-medley hostility scale,” Journal of Personality Assessment, vol. 65, no. 3, pp. 567–585, 1995.
[34]  R. Levin and G. Fireman, “Nightmare prevalence, nightmare distress, and self-reported psychological disturbance,” Sleep, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 205–212, 2002.
[35]  P. J. Brantley, C. D. Waggoner, G. N. Jones, and N. B. Rappaport, “A daily stress inventory: development, reliability, and validity,” Journal of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 61–74, 1987.
[36]  A. Kazdin, Research Design in Clinical Psychology, Allyn & Bacon, Boston, Mass, USA, 4th edition, 2002.
[37]  R. M. Baron and D. A. Kenny, “The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research. Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 51, no. 6, pp. 1173–1182, 1986.
[38]  R. G. Wilkinson, “Socioeconomic determinants of health: health inequalities: relative or absolute material standards?” British Medical Journal, vol. 314, no. 7080, pp. 591–595, 1997.
[39]  R. T. Gross and T. D. Borkovec, “Effects of cognitive intrusion manipulation on the sleep-onset latency of good sleepers,” Behavior Therapy, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 112–116, 1982.
[40]  E. S. Healey, A. Kales, and L. J. Monroe, “Onset of insomnia: role of life-stress events,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 43, no. 5, pp. 439–451, 1981.
[41]  N. J. Ali, D. Pitson, and J. R. Stradling, “Sleep disordered breathing: effects of adenotonsillectomy on behaviour and psychological functioning,” European Journal of Pediatrics, vol. 155, no. 1, pp. 56–62, 1996.
[42]  B. D. Booth, J. Paul Fedoroff, S. D. Curry, and A. B. Douglass, “Sleep apnea as a possible factor contributing to aggression in sex offenders,” Journal of Forensic Sciences, vol. 51, no. 5, pp. 1178–1181, 2006.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133