全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Visual Scanning Patterns during the Dimensional Change Card Sorting Task in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

DOI: 10.1155/2012/123053

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Impaired cognitive flexibility in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has been reported in previous literature. The present study explored ASD children’s visual scanning patterns during the Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) task using eye-tracking technique. ASD and typical developing (TD) children completed the standardized DCCS procedure on the computer while their eye movements were tracked. Behavioral results confirmed previous findings on ASD children’s deficits in executive function. ASD children’s visual scanning patterns also showed some specific underlying processes in the DCCS task compared to TD children. For example, ASD children looked shorter at the correct card in the postswitch phase and spent longer time at blank areas than TD children did. ASD children did not show a bias to the color dimension as TD children did. The correlations between the behavioral performance and eye moments were also discussed. 1. Introduction Executive dysfunction has been well studied and consistently found in previous studies in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [1–3]. Executive function (EF) refers to a wide range of abilities including planning, inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, impulse control, and so forth [1, 2, 4, 5]. The EF deficit in ASD has been found to be related to their symptoms of restricted and repetitive behaviors [6, 7], and their impairments in Theory of Mind [8–12]. Impaired EF has been found in ASD children in numerous previous studies, including the impaired planning skills in the Tower of Hanoi or the Tower of London task [13–17], the impaired visual spatial working memory [18–28] and verbal working memory [23, 29], the impaired inhibitory control in the Stroop task [20, 30, 31], and the deficit in cognitive flexibility in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST) [13, 32]. However, contradictory findings came from studies that did not find EF deficits in ASD, for example, in the working memory task [30] and in the WCST [31]. The Dimensional Change Card Sorting task, developed by Frye et al. [33], was widely used to measure the cognitive flexibility of typically developing (TD) preschoolers. Compared with the WCST, the DCCS task is relatively simple and suitable to use across a wide age range [34]. The DCCS task asks children to sort bidimensional test cards (e.g., a red rabbit and a blue boat) with one of the two target cards (e.g., a red boat and a blue rabbit) according to one rule (e.g., by color), and then after several successful trials, switch to another rule (e.g., by shape). Previous

References

[1]  E. L. Hill, “Executive dysfunction in autism,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 26–32, 2004.
[2]  B. F. Pennington and S. Ozonoff, “Executive functions and developmental psychopathology,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 37, no. 1, pp. 51–87, 1996.
[3]  J. A. Sergeant, H. Geurts, and J. Oosterlaan, “How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?” Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 130, no. 1-2, pp. 3–28, 2002.
[4]  A. Baddeley, Working Memory, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1986.
[5]  S. Ozonoff and D. L. Strayer, “Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 59–78, 1997.
[6]  J. Russell, Autism as an Executive Disorder, vol. 8, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1997.
[7]  M. Turner, “Repetitive behaviour in autism: a review of psychological research,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 40, no. 6, pp. 839–849, 1999.
[8]  E. Pellicano, “Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with Autism: clues to developmental primacy,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 974–990, 2007.
[9]  E. Colvert, D. Custance, and J. Swettenham, “Rule-based reasoning and theory of mind in Autism: a commentary on the work of Zelazo, jacques, burack and frye,” Infant and Child Development, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 197–200, 2002.
[10]  B. Cohen, T. Flusberg, and Cohen, Understanding other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 19, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2000.
[11]  N. Russo, T. Flanagan, G. Iarocci, D. Berringer, P. D. Zelazo, and J. A. Burack, “Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 77–86, 2007.
[12]  P. D. Zelazo, S. Jacques, J. A. Burack, and D. Frye, “The relation between theory of mind and rule use: evidence from persons with Autism-spectrum disorders,” Infant and Child Development, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 171–195, 2002.
[13]  C. Hughes, J. Russell, and T. W. Robbins, “Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 477–492, 1994.
[14]  H. M. Geurts, S. Verté, J. Oosterlaan, H. Roeyers, and J. A. Sergeant, “How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and autism?” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 4, pp. 836–854, 2004.
[15]  S. Ozonoff, B. F. Pennington, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 32, no. 7, pp. 1081–1105, 1991.
[16]  S. Ozonoff and R. E. McEvoy, “A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism,” Development and Psychopathology, vol. 6, pp. 415–431, 1994.
[17]  S. Ozonoff and J. Jensen, “Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 171–177, 1999.
[18]  M. C. Goldberg, S. H. Mostofsky, L. E. Cutting et al., “Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 279–293, 2005.
[19]  F. Happé, R. Booth, R. Charlton, and C. Hughes, “Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 25–39, 2006.
[20]  R. M. Joseph, S. D. Steele, E. Meyer, and H. Tager-Flusberg, “Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory?” Neuropsychologia, vol. 43, no. 10, pp. 1400–1411, 2005.
[21]  R. J. Landa and M. C. Goldberg, “Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: a continuum of performance,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 35, no. 5, pp. 557–573, 2005.
[22]  B. Luna, S. K. Doll, S. J. Hegedus, N. J. Minshew, and J. A. Sweeney, “Maturation of executive function in Autism,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 61, no. 4, pp. 474–481, 2007.
[23]  J. Russell, C. Jarrold, and L. Henry, “Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 673–686, 1996.
[24]  T. Reed, “Visual perspective taking as a measure of working memory in participants with autism,” Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 63–76, 2002.
[25]  S. D. Steele, N. J. Minshew, B. Luna, and J. A. Sweeney, “Spatial working memory deficits in autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 37, no. 4, pp. 605–612, 2007.
[26]  S. Verté, H. M. Geurts, H. Roeyers, J. Oosterlaan, and J. A. Sergeant, “Executive functioning in children with an autism spectrum disorder: can we differentiate within the spectrum?” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 36, no. 3, pp. 351–372, 2006.
[27]  D. L. Williams, G. Goldstein, P. A. Carpenter, and N. J. Minshew, “Verbal and spatial working memory in autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 35, no. 6, pp. 747–756, 2005.
[28]  D. L. Williams, G. Goldstein, and N. J. Minshew, “The profile of memory function in children with autism,” Neuropsychology, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 21–29, 2006.
[29]  C. S. Gabig, “Verbal working memory and story retelling in school-age children with autism,” Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, vol. 39, no. 4, pp. 498–511, 2008.
[30]  J. Cui, D. Gao, Y. Chen, X. Zou, and Y. Wang, “Working memory in early-school-age children with asperger's syndrome,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 40, no. 8, pp. 958–967, 2010.
[31]  A. Nydén, C. Gillberg, E. Hjelmquist, and M. Heiman, “Executive function/attention deficits in boys with Asperger syndrome, attention disorder and reading/writing disorder,” Autism, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 213–228, 1999.
[32]  S. Ozonoff, Autism as an Executive Disorder, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1997.
[33]  D. Frye, P. D. Zelazo, and T. Palfai, “Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning,” Cognitive Development, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 483–527, 1995.
[34]  P. D. Zelazo, “The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children,” Nature Protocols, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 297–301, 2006.
[35]  P. D. Zelazo, D. Frye, and T. Rapus, “An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them,” Cognitive Development, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 37–63, 1996.
[36]  P. D. Zelazo, U. Müller, D. Frye et al., “The development of executive function in early childhood,” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 1–153, 2003.
[37]  N. Chevalier and A. Blaye, “Setting goals to switch between tasks: effect of cue transparency on Children's cognitive flexibility,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 3, pp. 782–797, 2009.
[38]  D. Hongwanishkul, K. R. Happaney, W. S. C. Lee, and P. D. Zelazo, “Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 617–644, 2005.
[39]  G. S. Dichter, K. J. Radonovich, L. M. Turner-Brown, K. S. L. Lam, T. N. Holtzclaw, and J. W. Bodfish, “Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the dimension-change card sort task,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 40, no. 4, pp. 448–456, 2010.
[40]  N. Minar, “Visual Attention and the DCCS,” (Senior Honor Thesis), 2010, https://kb.osu.edu/dspace/handle/1811/45467.
[41]  N. Chevalier, A. Blaye, S. Dufau, and J. Lucenet, “What visual information do children and adults consider while switching between tasks? Eye-tracking investigation of cognitive flexibility development,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 46, no. 4, pp. 955–972, 2010.
[42]  M. R. Ellefson, L. R. Shapiro, and N. Chater, “Asymmetrical switch costs in children,” Cognitive Development, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 108–130, 2006.
[43]  L. Cragg and K. Nation, “Shifting development in mid-childhood: the influence of between-task interference,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 5, pp. 1465–1479, 2009.
[44]  B. Sang and X. Miao, “The revision of trial norm of Peabody picture vocabulary test revised (PPVT-R) In Shanghai proper,” Psychological Science, vol. 5, pp. 20–25, 1990 (Chinese).
[45]  American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC, USA, 4th edition, 1994.
[46]  B. Auyeung, S. Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, and C. Allison, “The autism spectrum quotient: Children's version (AQ-Child),” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 38, no. 7, pp. 1230–1240, 2008.
[47]  C. E. Wilson, J. Brock, and R. Palermo, “Attention to social stimuli and facial identity recognition skills in autism spectrum disorder,” Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, vol. 54, no. 12, pp. 1104–1115, 2010.
[48]  C. F. Norbury, J. Brock, L. Cragg, S. Einav, H. Griffiths, and K. Nation, “Eye-movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 50, no. 7, pp. 834–842, 2009.
[49]  J. A. Burack, G. Iarocci, T. D. Flanagan, and D. M. Bowler, “On mosaics and melting pots: conceptual considerations of comparison and matching strategies,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 65–73, 2004.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133