全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Activation of Right Ventral Prefrontal Cortex Using a Predictive Cue during Visual Spatial Orienting of Attentional Processes: An fMRI Study

DOI: 10.1155/2012/961342

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Visual spatial orienting of attention can be investigated with location-cueing paradigms in which a cue provides correct information about the location of the upcoming target. Target detection is facilitated when the target appears at the expected cued location. In this study, we examined the brain activation of the spatial orienting response based on attentional “benefits.” During an fMRI experiment, two types of attentional tasks were used. Both predictive and nonpredictive cues were used and followed by an upcoming target. Behavioral data showed a faster reaction time with the predictive cue when compared with that of the nonpredictive cue. The fMRI results of these two tasks were compared, whereby isolated brain areas activated when the targets appeared at the attended position after a specific spatial expectation was induced by the cue were compared with when equivalent targets appeared after no spatial expectation was induced by the cue. The results showed that the right ventral prefrontal cortex was activated to a similar degree as the dorsal frontoparietal spatial attentional network. 1. Introduction The visual spatial orienting of attention experimental paradigm is a widely studied model that was first developed by Posner [1] for studying the covert visual spatial orienting of attention. In this paradigm, an arrow can usually be used as a cue and presented in the center of the visual field. When a cue is presented, it points left or right in order to provide a spatial hint for the upcoming target, whereby the participants can predict the location of a target and pay attention to that location voluntarily. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have discovered a dorsal frontoparietal network, which includes the inferior parietal lobe (IPL) and the frontal eye field (FEF), and this network is involved in visual spatial orienting of attention [2–4]. In these goal-directed attention paradigms (spatial endogenous orienting of attention paradigm), a visual cue is usually presented centrally and provides a spatial prediction of the upcoming target [5]. When a cue provides the spatial hint correctly, it is called valid cue. When a cue provides the wrong spatial hint, it is called an invalid cue. The percentage of valid cue counts is called validity. By using a predictive cue with 100% validity [5], that is, a central visual spatial cue that indicates the location of an upcoming peripheral target, stimulus detection is facilitated when the target appears at the expected (i.e., the validly cued) location. The difference in reaction times

References

[1]  M. I. Posner, “Orienting of attention,” The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 3–25, 1980.
[2]  M. Corbetta and G. L. Shulman, “Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 201–215, 2002.
[3]  D. V. Smith, B. Davis, K. Niu et al., “Spatial attention evokes similar activation patterns for visual and auditory stimuli,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 347–361, 2010.
[4]  E. I. Knudsen, “Fundamental components of attention,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 30, pp. 57–78, 2007.
[5]  J. T. Coull and A. C. Nobre, “Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 18, no. 18, pp. 7426–7435, 1998.
[6]  F. Doricchi, E. MacCi, M. Silvetti, and E. MacAluso, “Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the posner task,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 20, no. 7, pp. 1574–1585, 2010.
[7]  J. V. Pardo, P. T. Fox, and M. E. Raichle, “Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography,” Nature, vol. 349, no. 6304, pp. 61–64, 1991.
[8]  W. Sturm, A. de Simone, B. J. Krause et al., “Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 37, no. 7, pp. 797–805, 1999.
[9]  G. L. Shulman, D. L. W. Pope, S. V. Astafiev, M. P. McAvoy, A. Z. Snyder, and M. Corbetta, “Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 10, pp. 3640–3651, 2010.
[10]  D. S. Rizzuto, A. N. Mamelak, W. W. Sutherling, I. Fineman, and R. A. Andersen, “Spatial selectivity in human ventrolateral prefrontal cortex,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 415–417, 2005.
[11]  K. J. Friston, P. Fletcher, O. Josephs, A. Holmes, M. D. Rugg, and R. Turner, “Event-related fMRI: characterizing differential responses,” NeuroImage, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 30–40, 1998.
[12]  K. J. Friston, W. Penny, C. Phillips, S. Kiebel, G. Hinton, and J. Ashburner, “Classical and Bayesian inference in neuroimaging: theory,” NeuroImage, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 465–483, 2002.
[13]  C. Büchel, O. Josephs, G. Rees, R. Turner, C. D. Frith, and K. J. Friston, “The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study,” Brain, vol. 121, no. 7, pp. 1281–1294, 1998.
[14]  M. Behrmann, J. J. Geng, and S. Shomstein, “Parietal cortex and attention,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 212–217, 2004.
[15]  T. Grent-'t-Jong and M. G. Woldorff, “Timing and sequence of brain activity in top-down control of visual-spatial attention,” PLoS Biology, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 114–126, 2007.
[16]  J. B. Hopfinger, M. H. Buonocore, and G. R. Mangun, “The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 284–291, 2000.
[17]  A. R. Mayer, A. R. Franco, and D. L. Harrington, “Neuronal modulation of auditory attention by informative and uninformative spatial cues,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 1652–1666, 2009.
[18]  T. A. Kelley, J. T. Serences, B. Giesbrecht, and S. Yantis, “Cortical mechanisms for shifting and holding visuospatial attention,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 114–125, 2008.
[19]  A. S. Greenberg, M. Esterman, D. Wilson, J. T. Serences, and S. Yantis, “Control of spatial and feature-based attention in frontoparietal cortex,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 43, pp. 14330–14339, 2010.
[20]  T. Hedden and J. D. E. Gabrieli, “The ebb and flow of attention in the human brain,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 9, no. 7, pp. 863–865, 2006.
[21]  A. R. Mayer, D. Harrington, J. C. Adair, and R. Lee, “The neural networks underlying endogenous auditory covert orienting and reorienting,” NeuroImage, vol. 30, no. 3, pp. 938–949, 2006.
[22]  S. Shomstein and S. Yantis, “Control of attention shifts between vision and audition in human cortex,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 24, no. 47, pp. 10702–10706, 2004.
[23]  V. Santangelo, S. Fagioli, and E. Macaluso, “The costs of monitoring simultaneously two sensory modalities decrease when dividing attention in space,” NeuroImage, vol. 49, no. 3, pp. 2717–2727, 2010.
[24]  A. Baddeley, “Working memory: looking back and looking forward,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 4, no. 10, pp. 829–839, 2003.
[25]  C. Ranganath and M. D'Esposito, “Directing the mind's eye: prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 175–182, 2005.
[26]  K. Sakai, “Task set and prefrontal cortex,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 31, pp. 219–245, 2008.
[27]  S. Vossel, C. M. Thiel, and G. R. Fink, “Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex,” NeuroImage, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 1257–1264, 2006.
[28]  G. Aston-Jones and F. E. Bloom, “Norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats exhibit pronounced responses to non-noxious environmental stimuli,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 1, no. 8, pp. 887–900, 1981.
[29]  G. Aston-Jones and F. E. Bloom, “Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 1, no. 8, pp. 876–886, 1981.
[30]  M. I. Posner and S. E. Petersen, “The attention system of the human brain,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 13, pp. 25–42, 1990.
[31]  S. Bouret and S. J. Sara, “Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 28, no. 11, pp. 574–582, 2005.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413