全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

A Manipulation of Visual Feedback during Gait Training in Parkinson's Disease

DOI: 10.1155/2012/508720

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Visual cues are known to improve gait in Parkinson's disease (PD); however, the contribution of optic flow continues to be disputed. This study manipulated transverse line cues during two gait training interventions (6 weeks). PD subjects ( ) were assigned to one of three groups: treadmill (TG), overground (OG), or control group (CG). Participants walked across lines placed on either treadmills or 16-meter carpets, respectively. The treadmill (TG) offered a reduced dynamic flow from the environment, while lines presented on the ground (OG) emphasized optic flow related to the participant's own displacement. Both interventions significantly improved (and maintained through retention period) step length, thus improving walking velocity. Only the OG improved in the TUG test, while only the TG showed hints of improving (and maintaining) motor symptoms. Since gait improvements were found in both training groups, we conclude that by reducing optic flow, gait benefits associated with visual cueing training can still be achieved. 1. Introduction Individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) have been shown to walk with a stooped posture, limited arm swing, slow velocity, and small shuffling steps that can often lead to falls [1]. Sensory cueing strategies such as auditory, tactile, and visual cues have often been used to help walking in PD. Stein and Glickstein [2] suggested that of all these modalities, visual cues are most effective in improving PD gait. It is not clear, however, whether improvements might be the result of improved use of optic flow, greater attention directed towards walking, or cortically driven planning of discrete steps that bypass the basal ganglia. Optic flow is a prominent theory that is often put forward to explain the benefits associated with using transverse lines. This theory suggests that transverse lines improve walking due to the stripes accentuating the flow of the surrounding environment as one moves through space [3, 4]. This notion of optic flow has been strongly supported by Azulay et al. [5] that believe the lines emphasized optic flow which improved gait velocity and stride length in PD participants. Optic flow has been previously manipulated through either virtual reality or a projected tunnel screen [6, 7], and in each case, manipulation was presented by changing the surrounding environment. An interesting method of manipulating visual information from the surrounding environment is to have people walk on a treadmill. Biomechanically, the differences that exist between treadmill and overground walking are negligible [8].

References

[1]  M. E. Morris, R. Iansek, T. A. Matyas, and J. J. Summers, “Ability to modulate walking cadence remains intact in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 57, no. 12, pp. 1532–1534, 1994.
[2]  M. Glickstein and J. Stein, “Paradoxical movement in Parkinson's disease,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 14, no. 11, pp. 480–482, 1991.
[3]  C. Assaiante, A. R. Marchand, and B. Amblard, “Discrete visual samples may control locomotor equilibrium and foot positioning in man,” Journal of Motor Behavior, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 72–91, 1989.
[4]  J. J. Gibson, “The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement,” Psychological Review, vol. 61, no. 5, pp. 304–314, 1954.
[5]  J. P. Azulay, S. Mesure, B. Amblard, O. Blin, I. Sangla, and J. Pouget, “Visual control of locomotion in Parkinson's disease,” Brain, vol. 122, part 1, pp. 111–120, 1999.
[6]  T. Prokop, M. Schubert, and W. Berger, “Visual influence on human locomotion. Modulation to changes in optic flow,” Experimental Brain Research, vol. 114, no. 1, pp. 63–70, 1997.
[7]  A. Lamontagne, J. Fung, B. J. McFadyen, and J. Faubert, “Modulation of walking speed by changing optic flow in persons with stroke,” Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, vol. 4, article 22, 2007.
[8]  J. L. Song and J. Hidler, “Biomechanics of overground vs. treadmill walking in healthy individuals,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 104, no. 3, pp. 747–755, 2008.
[9]  S. Frenkel-Toledo, N. Giladi, C. Peretz, T. Herman, L. Gruendlinger, and J. M. Hausdorff, “Effect of gait speed on gait rhythmicity in Parkinson's disease: variability of stride time and swing time respond differently,” Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, vol. 2, article 23, 2005.
[10]  O. Bello, J. A. Sanchez, and M. Fernandez-del-Olmo, “Treadmill walking in Parkinson's disease patients: adaptation and generalization effect,” Movement Disorders, vol. 23, no. 9, pp. 1243–1249, 2008.
[11]  M. Suteerawattananon, G. S. Morris, B. R. Etnyre, J. Jankovic, and E. J. Protas, “Effects of visual and auditory cues on gait in individuals with Parkinson's disease,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences, vol. 219, no. 1-2, pp. 63–69, 2004.
[12]  C. A. Lebold and Q. J. Almeida, “An evaluation of mechanisms underlying the influence of step cues on gait in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 18, no. 6, pp. 798–802, 2011.
[13]  C. A. Lebold and Q. J. Almeida, “Evaluating the contributions of dynamic flow to freezing of gait in parkinson's disease,” Parkinson's Disease, Article ID 732508, 2010.
[14]  A. Nieuwboer, “Cueing for freezing of gait in patients with Parkinson's disease: a rehabilitation perspective,” Movement Disorders, vol. 23, supplement 2, pp. S475–S481, 2008.
[15]  J. Purdon Martin, “The basal ganglia and postural mechanisms,” Agressologie, vol. 18, pp. 75–81, 1977.
[16]  Y. Jiang and K. E. Norman, “Effects of visual and auditory cues on gait initiation in people with Parkinson's disease,” Clinical Rehabilitation, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 36–45, 2006.
[17]  M. E. Morris, R. Iansek, T. A. Matyas, and J. J. Summers, “Stride length regulation in Parkinson's disease: normalization strategies and underlying mechanisms,” Brain, vol. 119, part 2, pp. 551–568, 1996.
[18]  E. van Wegen, I. Lim, C. De Goede et al., “The effects of visual rhythms and optic flow on stride patterns of patients with Parkinson's disease,” Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 21–27, 2006.
[19]  B. Sidaway, J. Anderson, G. Danielson, L. Martin, and G. Smith, “Effects of long-term gait training using visual cues in an individual with Parkinson disease,” Physical Therapy, vol. 86, no. 2, pp. 186–194, 2006.
[20]  D. Podsiadlo and S. Richardson, “The timed “Up and Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 39, no. 2, pp. 142–148, 1991.
[21]  T. Hanakawa, H. Fukuyama, Y. Katsumi, M. Honda, and H. Shibasaki, “Enhanced lateral premotor activity during paradoxical gait in parkinson's disease,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 45, no. 3, pp. 329–336, 1999.
[22]  R. Marchese, et al., “The role of sensory cues in the rehabilitation of Parkinsonian patients: a comparison of two physical therapy protocols,” Movement Disorders, vol. 15, no. 5, pp. 879–883, 2000.
[23]  M. Ferrarin, M. Rabuffetti, M. Tettamanti, R. Pignatti, A. Mauro, and G. Albani, “Effect of optical flow versus attentional strategy on gait in Parkinson's disease: a study with a portable optical stimulating device,” Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, vol. 5, article 3, 2008.
[24]  T. Platz, R. G. Brown, and C. D. Marsden, “Training improves the speed of aimed movements in Parkinson's disease,” Brain, vol. 121, part 3, pp. 505–514, 1998.
[25]  Y. Masuda, Y. Nisida, and K. Kurosawa, “Relationship of a 30-second chair-stand test to gait performance in stroke patients,” Rigakuryoho Kagaku, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 69–73, 2004.
[26]  A. R. Lindquist, C. L. Prado, R. M. L. Barros, R. Mattioli, P. H. Lobo Da Costa, and T. F. Salvini, “Gait training combining partial body-weight support, a treadmill, and functional electrical stimulation: effects on poststroke gait,” Physical Therapy, vol. 87, no. 9, pp. 1144–1154, 2007.
[27]  T. Toole, C. G. Maitland, E. Warren, M. F. Hubmann, and L. Panton, “The effects of loading and unloading treadmill walking on balance, gait, fall risk, and daily function in Parkinsonism,” NeuroRehabilitation, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 307–322, 2005.
[28]  S. Bennie, et al., “Measurements of balance: comparison of the timed “Up and Go” test and functional reach test with the berg balance scale,” Journal of Physical Therapy Science, no. 15, pp. 93–97, 2003.
[29]  J. Carr and R. B. Shepard, Neurological Rehabilitation: Optimizing Motor Performance, The Bath Press, Kent, 1998.
[30]  E. Stack and A. Ashburn, “Fall events described by people with Parkinson's disease: implications for clinical interviewing and the research agenda,” Physiotherapy Research International, vol. 4, no. 3, pp. 190–200, 1999.
[31]  S. Rahman, H. J. Griffin, N. P. Quinn, and M. Jahanshahi, “The factors that induce or overcome freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease,” Behavioural Neurology, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 127–136, 2008.
[32]  G. Alves, T. Wentzel-Larsen, D. Aarsland, and J. P. Larsen, “Progression of motor impairment and disability in Parkinson disease: a population-based study,” Neurology, vol. 65, no. 9, pp. 1436–1441, 2005.
[33]  O. Bello, G. Marquez, M. Camblor, and M. Fernandez-Del-Olmo, “Mechanisms involved in treadmill walking improvements in Parkinson's disease,” Gait and Posture, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 118–123, 2010.
[34]  G. Abbruzzese and A. Berardelli, “Sensorimotor integration in movement disorders,” Movement Disorders, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 231–240, 2003.
[35]  R. Meeusen, M. F. Piacentini, and K. De Meirleir, “Brain microdialysis in exercise research,” Sports Medicine, vol. 31, no. 14, pp. 965–983, 2001.
[36]  P. G. MacRae, W. W. Spirduso, and T. J. Walters, “Endurance training effects on striatal D2 dopamine receptor binding and striatal dopamine metabolites in presenescent older rats,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 92, no. 2, pp. 236–240, 1987.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133