全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Differences in Architects and Nonarchitects' Perception of Urban Design: An Application of Kansei Engineering Techniques

DOI: 10.1155/2011/736307

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

We analyse architects and nonarchitects' emotional assessments of different districts in their own city (Valencia, Spain) by applying Kansei engineering techniques. A field study was carried out on a sample of 140 subjects (70 architects and 70 nonarchitects) who were asked to express their opinions on different areas in the city. The set of emotional impressions used by architects and non-architects to describe their sensations was obtained using differential semantics. The semantic space was described by 9 independent axis which explained 62% of the variability. Then, for each collective the set of impressions which influence the final residential or investment area decision was analysed. This relationship was obtained applying linear regression models. The results showed no significant differences between both groups so that the emotional attributes determining the choice of area were very similar for architects and non-architects. Greater discrepancies were found when the purpose of the choice was investment and not residential. Finally a neighbourhood was semantically profiled to represent and compare both collectives' perceptions. 1. Introduction There are many studies on the differences in architects and nonarchitects’ evaluations [1–13]. Some studies have analysed the categories that both collectives use when assessing buildings. Groat [2], for example, studied the differences in architects and economists’ perceptions and found that while economists classified housing on the basis of type, architects used quality of design, form, style, and historical significance categories. In this regard, in an analysis of two office blocks in Chicago, Devlin [7] observed that whereas nonarchitects tended to give descriptive assessments based on affection, architects provided conceptual, more abstract assessments. Other studies have focused on the different assessments of architectural styles. Gans [1] observed that architects preferred “high” style over “popular”, the style preferred by nonexperts. Subsequently, Devlin and Nasar [6] concluded that this was due to the different emotional assessments which the styles caused in both collectives. For nonexperts the “popular” style was coherent, pleasurable, and clean, signifiers which architects attributed to the “high” style. Fewer works have attempted to identify what specific design elements cause the different assessments. In this regard, Gifford et al. [11] analysed the relationship between the physical characteristics of buildings, the perceptions “attractive” and “pleasurable,” and the subject’s overall

References

[1]  H. Gans, “Towards a human architecture: a sociologist view of the profession,” Journal of Architectural Education, vol. 2, pp. 26–31, 1978.
[2]  L. N. Groat, “Meaning in post-modern architecture: an examination using the multiple sorting task,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 3–22, 1982.
[3]  C. Friedman, J. D. Balling, and J. J. Valadez, “Visual preference for office buildings: a comparison of architects and non-architects,” in Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Environmental Design Research Association, 1985.
[4]  M. Duffy, S. Bailey, B. Beck, and D. G. Barker, “Preferences in nursing home design: a comparison of residents, administrators and designers,” Environment and Behavior, vol. 18, pp. 246–257, 1986.
[5]  J. C. Vischer and C. C. Marcus, “Evaluating evaluation: analysis of a housing design awards program,” Places, vol. 3, pp. 66–86, 1986.
[6]  K. Devlin and J. Nasar, “The beauty and the beast: some preliminary comparisons of "high" versus "popular" residential architecture and public versus architect judgments of same,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, vol. 9, no. 4, pp. 333–344, 1989.
[7]  K. Devlin, “An examination of architectural interpretation: architects versus nonarchitects,” in Design Review: Challenging Urban Aesthetic Control, B. C. Scheer and W. F. E. Preiser, Eds., pp. 156–164, Chapman & Hall, New York, NY, USA, 1990.
[8]  J. L. Nasar and T. Purcell, “Beauty and the beast extended: knowledge structure and evaluations of houses by australian architects and non-architects,” in Proceedings of the Culture, Space, History. Sevki Vanh Foundation for Architecture, 1990.
[9]  T. Purcell and J. L. Nasar, “Australian architect and non-architect experiences of american houses,” in Proceedings of the 11th International Congress on Empirical Aesthetics of the International Association of Empirical Aesthetics, 1990.
[10]  A. E. Stamps, “Public preferences for high rise buildings: stylistic and demographic effects,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 72, pp. 839–844, 1991.
[11]  R. Gifford, D. W. Hine, W. Muller-Clemm, D. Reynolds, and K. Shaw, “Decoding modern architecture: a lens model approach for understanding the aesthetic differences of architects and laypersons,” Environment and Behavior, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 163–187, 2000.
[12]  R. Gifford, D. W. Hine, W. Muller-Clemm, and K. T. Shaw, “Why architects and laypersons judge buildings differently: cognitive properties and physical bases,” Journal of Architectural and Planning Research, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 131–148, 2002.
[13]  A. Akalin, K. Yildirim, C. Wilson, and O. Kilicoglu, “Architecture and engineering students' evaluations of house fa?ades: preference, complexity and impressiveness,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, vol. 29, no. 1, pp. 124–132, 2009.
[14]  E. Brunswik, Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments, University of California, Berkeley, Calif, USA, 1956.
[15]  D. E. Berlyne, Aesthetics and Psychobiology, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, NY, USA, 1971.
[16]  J. L. Nasar, “Architectural symbolism: a study of house-style meanings,” in Proceedings of the Paths to Co-existence of Environmental Design Research Association (EDRA '88), D. Lawrence and B. Wasserman, Eds., vol. 19, pp. 63–171, 1988.
[17]  A. T. Purcell, “Environmental perception and affect: a schema discrepancy model,” Environment and Behavior, vol. 18, pp. 3–30, 1986.
[18]  A. T. Purcell, “The relationship between buildings and behaviour,” Building and Environment, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 215–232, 1987.
[19]  A. T. Purcell and J. L. Nasar, “Experiencing other people's houses: a model of similarities and differences in environmental experience,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 199–211, 1992.
[20]  P. Lewicki, T. Hill, and E. Bizot, “Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 24–27, 1988.
[21]  L. W. Barsalou, “Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 629–654, 1985.
[22]  F. M. Dieleman and C. H. Mulder, “The geography of residential choice,” in Residential Environments: Choice, Satisfaction and Behavior, J. I. Aragonés, G. Francescato, and T. G?rling , Eds., pp. 35–54, Bergin & Garvey, South Hadley, Mass, USA, 2002.
[23]  C. H. Mulder, “Housing choice: assumptions and approaches,” Journal of Housing and the Built Environment, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 209–232, 1996.
[24]  I. G. Ellen and M. A. Turner, “Does neighborhood matter? Assessing recent evidence,” Housing Policy Debate, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 833–866, 1997.
[25]  T. G?rling and M. Friman, “A psychological conceptualization of residencial choice and satisfaction,” in Residential Environments: Choice, Satisfaction and Behavior, J. I. Aragonés, G. Francescato, and T. G?rling, Eds., pp. 55–80, Bergin & Garvey, South Hadley, Mass, USA, 2002.
[26]  M. Nagamachi, Kansei Engineering, Kaibundo, Tokyo, Japan, 1989.
[27]  M. Nagamachi, “Kansei engineering: a new ergonomic consumer-oriented technology for product development,” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 3–11, 1995.
[28]  K. Lynch, A Theory of Good City Form, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1981.
[29]  D. Appleyard, Livable Streets, University of California, Berkeley, Calif, USA, 1981.
[30]  F. D. Jarvis, Site Planning and Community Design for Great Neighborhoods, Home Builder, Washington, DC, USA, 1993.
[31]  R. Küller, “Architecture and emotions,” in Architecture for People, B. Milkellides, Ed., pp. 87–100, Studio Vista, London, UK, 1980.
[32]  R. Küller, “Environmental assessment from a neuropsychological perspective,” in Environment Cognition and Action: An Integrated Approach, T. G?rling and G. W. Evans, Eds., pp. 111–147, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1991.
[33]  A. P. Field, Discovering Statistics Using SPSS, Sage, London, UK, 2nd edition, 2005.
[34]  J. Terninko, Step-by-Step QFD: Costumer-Driven Product Design, St. Lucie Press, Florida, 1997.
[35]  C. E. Osgood, G. J. Suci, and P. H. Tannenbaum, The Measurement of Meaning, University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ill, USA, 1957.
[36]  T. Jindo, K. Hirasago, and M. Nagamachi, “Development of a design support system for office chairs using 3-D graphics,” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 49–62, 1995.
[37]  Y. Matsubara and M. Nagamachi, “Hybrid Kansei Engineering System and design support,” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 81–92, 1997.
[38]  C. Llinares and A. F. Page, “Application of product differential semantics to quantify purchaser perceptions in housing assessment,” Building and Environment, vol. 42, no. 7, pp. 2488–2497, 2007.
[39]  B. Flury, Common Principal Components and Related Multivariate Models, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA, 1988.
[40]  A. Basilevsky, Statistical Factor Analysis and Related Methods: Theory and Applications, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA, 1994.
[41]  D. L. Streiner, “Starting at the beginning: an introduction to coefficient alpha and internal consistency,” Journal of Personality Assessment, vol. 80, no. 1, pp. 99–103, 2003.
[42]  S. Nagasawa, “Kansei evaluation using fuzzy structural modeling,” in Proceedings of the 1st Japan-Korea symposium on kansei engineering—consumer oriented product development technology on Kansei engineering I, M. Nagamachi, Ed., pp. 119–125, Kaibundo, 1997.
[43]  C. Llinares and A. F. Page, “Differential semantics as a Kansei Engineering tool for analysing the emotional impressions which determine the choice of neighbourhood: the case of Valencia, Spain,” Landscape and Urban Planning, vol. 87, no. 4, pp. 247–257, 2008.
[44]  T. R. Herzog, “A cognitive analysis of preference for urban spaces,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 237–248, 1992.
[45]  R. Kaplan, “The role of nature in the urban context,” in Human Behavior and Environment: Advances in Theory and Research, I. Altman and J. F. Wohlwill, Eds., vol. 6, pp. 127–162, Plenum Press, New York, NY, USA, 1983.
[46]  J. L. Nasar, “Perception and evaluation of residential street scenes,” in Environmental Aesthetics: Theory, Research and Applications, J. L. Nasar, Ed., pp. 228–253, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1988.
[47]  J. L. Nasar, “The evaluative image of the city,” Journal of the American Planning Association, vol. 56, no. 1, pp. 41–53, 1990.
[48]  T. Jindo and K. Hirasago, “Application studies to car interior of Kansei engineering,” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 105–114, 1997.
[49]  S. Ishihara, K. Ishihara, M. Nagamachi, and Y. Matsubara, “An analysis of Kansei structure on shoes using self-organizing neural networks,” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 93–104, 1997.
[50]  Y. Shimizu and T. Jindo, “A fuzzy logic analysis method for evaluating human sensitivities,” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 39–47, 1995.

Full-Text

Contact Us

[email protected]

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133