全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Interpretation of Melanoma Risk Feedback in First-Degree Relatives of Melanoma Patients

DOI: 10.1155/2012/374842

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Little is known about how individuals might interpret brief genetic risk feedback. We examined interpretation and behavioral intentions (sun protection, skin screening) in melanoma first-degree relatives (FDRs) after exposure to brief prototypic melanoma risk feedback. Using a 3 by 2 experimental pre-post design where feedback type (high-risk mutation, gene environment, and nongenetic) and risk level (positive versus negative findings) were systematically varied, 139 melanoma FDRs were randomized to receive one of the six scenarios. All scenarios included an explicit reminder that melanoma family history increased their risk regardless of their feedback. The findings indicate main effects by risk level but not feedback type; positive findings led to heightened anticipated melanoma risk perceptions and anticipated behavioral intentions. Yet those who received negative findings often discounted their family melanoma history. As such, 25%, 30%, and 32% of those who received negative mutation, gene-environment, and nongenetic feedback, respectively, reported that their risk was similar to the general population. Given the frequency with which those who pursue genetic testing may receive negative feedback, attention is needed to identify ideal strategies to present negative genetic findings in contexts such as direct to consumer channels where extensive genetic counseling is not required. 1. Background The sequencing of the entire human genome in 2003 has led to a series of unrealized opportunities for public health benefit [1], many of which rest on accurate genetic risk interpretation and adoption of protective behavior [2]. By 2006, direct-to-consumer genetic testing and feedback was available through 24 Internet-based companies, many of which did not require physician or genetic counseling followup to ensure accurate interpretation of test findings [3]. Recent general population surveys indicate high levels of risk misinterpretation even among highly educated general population subgroups [4–6]. To date, the few studies that have examined outcomes associated with direct-to-consumer genetic testing have found no remarkable increases in distress, screening, or behavior change [7–9], yet it is unclear whether these findings may be due to risk misinterpretation, or lack of consideration of diverse elements of risk, including family history. First-degree relatives (FDRs) of cancer patients may be among the first to pursue cancer genetic susceptibility testing through direct-to-consumer channels, given their heightened risk salience [10]. Among FDRs,

References

[1]  F. S. Collins, E. D. Green, A. E. Guttmacher, and M. S. Guyer, “A vision for the future of genomics research,” Nature, vol. 422, no. 6934, pp. 835–847, 2003.
[2]  M. J. Khoury, S. B. Clauser, A. N. Freedman, et al., “Population sciences, translational research, and the opportunities and challenges for genomics to reduce the burden of cancer in the 21st century,” Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers and Prevention, vol. 20, no. 10, pp. 2105–2114, 2011.
[3]  R. Geransar and E. Einsiedel, “Evaluating online direct-to-consumer marketing of genetic tests: informed choices or buyers beware,” Genetic Testing, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 13–23, 2008.
[4]  S. E. Gollust, E. S. Gordon, C. Zayac et al., “Motivations and perceptions of early adopters of personalized genomics: perspectives from research participants,” Public Health Genomics, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 22–30, 2012.
[5]  J. W. Leighton, K. Valverde, and B. A. Bernhardt, “The general public's understanding and perception of direct-to-consumer genetic test results,” Public Health Genomics, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 11–21, 2012.
[6]  A. L. McGuire, C. M. Diaz, T. Wang, and S. G. Hilsenbeck, “Social networkers' attitudes toward direct-to-consumer personal genome testing,” The American Journal of Bioethics, vol. 9, no. 6-7, pp. 3–10, 2009.
[7]  C. S. Bloss, N. J. Schork, and E. J. Topol, “Effect of direct-to-consumer genomewide profiling to assess disease risk,” The New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 364, no. 6, pp. 524–534, 2011.
[8]  K. M. James, C. T. Cowl, J. C. Tilburt, et al., “Impact of direct-to-consumer predictive genomic testing on risk perception and worry among patients receiving routine care in a preventive health clinic,” Mayo Clinic Proceedings, vol. 86, no. 10, pp. 933–940, 2011.
[9]  K. A. Kaphingst, C. M. McBride, C. Wade, et al., “Patients' understanding of and responses to multiplex genetic susceptibility test results,” Genetics in Medicine. In press.
[10]  C. M. McBride, K. M. Emmons, and I. M. Lipkus, “Understanding the potential of teachable moments: the case of smoking cessation,” Health Education Research, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 156–170, 2003.
[11]  S. LaRusse, J. S. Roberts, T. M. Marteau et al., “Genetic susceptibility testing versus family history-based risk assessment: impact on perceived risk of Alzheimer disease,” Genetics in Medicine, vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 48–53, 2005.
[12]  A. L. Ersig, D. W. Hadley, and L. M. Koehly, “Understanding patterns of health communication in families at risk for hereditary nonpolyposis colorectal cancer: examining the effect of conclusive versus indeterminate genetic test results,” Health Communication, vol. 26, no. 7, pp. 587–594, 2011.
[13]  A. L. Ersig, L. Ayres, D. W. Hadley, and L. M. Koehly, “Explanations of risk in families without identified mutations for hereditary nonpolyposis colorectal cancer,” Journal of Nursing Scholarship, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 139–146, 2010.
[14]  I. M. Lipkus, C. M. McBride, K. I. Pollak, P. Lyna, and G. Bepler, “Interpretation of genetic risk feedback among African American smokers with low socioeconomic status,” Health Psychology, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 178–188, 2004.
[15]  S. L. Docherty, C. M. McBride, S. C. Sanderson, S. C. O'Neill, J. A. Shepperd, and I. M. Lipkus, “Young smokers' views of genetic susceptibility testing for lung cancer risk: minding unintended consequences,” Journal of Community Genetics, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 165–172, 2011.
[16]  J. L. Hay, H. W. Meischke, D. J. Bowen et al., “Anticipating dissemination of cancer genomics in public health: a theoretical approach to psychosocial and behavioral challenges,” Annals of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 275–286, 2007.
[17]  M. Bergenmar and Y. Brandberg, “Sunbathing and sun-protection behaviors and attitudes of young Swedish adults with hereditary risk for malignant melanoma,” Cancer Nursing, vol. 24, no. 5, pp. 341–350, 2001.
[18]  T. B. Fitzpatrick, “The validity and practicality of sun-reactive skin types I through VI,” Archives of Dermatology, vol. 124, no. 6, pp. 869–871, 1988.
[19]  F. A. d Snoo, W. Bergman, and N. A. Gruis, “Familial melanoma: a complex disorder leading to controversy on DNA testing,” Familial Cancer, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 109–116, 2003.
[20]  S. Raimondi, F. Sera, S. Gandini et al., “MC1R variants, melanoma and red hair color phenotype: a meta-analysis,” International Journal of Cancer, vol. 122, no. 12, pp. 2753–2760, 2008.
[21]  K. Glanz, E. Schoenfeld, M. A. Weinstock, G. Layi, J. Kidd, and D. M. Shigaki, “Development and reliability of a brief skin cancer risk assessment tool,” Cancer Detection and Prevention, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 311–315, 2003.
[22]  S. Persky, K. A. Kaphingst, C. M. Condit, and C. M. McBride, “Assessing hypothetical scenario methodology in genetic susceptibility testing analog studies: a quantitative review,” Genetics in Medicine, vol. 9, no. 11, pp. 727–738, 2007.
[23]  N. D. Weinstein, “Unrealistic optimism about future life events,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 39, no. 5, pp. 806–820, 1980.
[24]  K. Glanz, A. L. Yaroch, M. Dancel et al., “Measures of sun exposure and sun protection practices for behavioral and epidemiologic research,” Archives of Dermatology, vol. 144, no. 2, pp. 217–222, 2008.
[25]  S. W. Raudenbush and A. S. Bryk, Hierarchical Linear Models, vol. 1, Sage Publications, Newbury Park, Calif, USA, 2nd edition, 2002.
[26]  S. L. Zeger and K. Y. Liang, “Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes,” Biometrics, vol. 42, no. 1, pp. 121–130, 1986.
[27]  P. Peduzzi, J. Concato, E. Kemper, T. R. Holford, and A. R. Feinstem, “A simulation study of the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis,” Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, vol. 49, no. 12, pp. 1373–1379, 1996.
[28]  M. Conner and P. Norman, Predicting Health Behaviour: Research and Practice with Social Cognition Models, Open University Press, Buckingham, UK, 2nd edition, 2005.
[29]  L. D. Cameron, K. A. Sherman, T. M. Marteau, and P. M. Brown, “Impact of genetic risk information and type of disease on perceived risk, anticipated affect, and expected consequences of genetic tests,” Health Psychology, vol. 28, no. 3, pp. 307–316, 2009.
[30]  L. D. Cameron, T. M. Marteau, P. M. Brown, W. M. P. Klein, and K. A. Sherman, “Communication strategies for enhancing understanding of the behavioral implications of genetic and biomarker tests for disease risk: the role of coherence,” Journal of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 286–298, 2012.
[31]  K. D. Graves, B. N. Peshkin, G. Luta, W. Tuong, and M. D. Schwartz, “Interest in genetic testing for modest changes in breast cancer risk: implicationsfor SNP testing,” Public Health Genomics, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 178–189, 2011.
[32]  R. G. Crowder, Principles of Learning and Memory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1976.
[33]  D. Ford, J. M. Bliss, A. J. Swerdlow et al., “Risk of cutaneous melanoma associated with a family history of the disease,” International Journal of Cancer, vol. 62, no. 4, pp. 377–381, 1995.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133