全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Naturalizing Semantics? Beyond Cognitive Neuro-Reductionism: From Varela’s Systemic Cognitive Neuroscience to Complex Realism Sociology

DOI: 10.4236/sm.2022.122003, PP. 29-57

Keywords: Cognitive Neuro-Reductionism, Free Will and Responsibility, Enactivism, Neurophenomenology, Complex Realism Sociology

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Is it possible to naturalize semantics? Starting from Libet’s 1983 studies, current research developments in neuronal bases of behavior reduce the mind to the brain, with significant implications in reference to issues of free will, imputability and individual behavioral responsibility. However, many criticisms can be made at this approach. This paper shows the limits of Cognitive Neuro-reductionism, especially in the light of Varela’s Systemic Cognitive Neuroscience or Neurophenomenology and the current theoretical revision process of social systems as complexdynamical, emergent and unpredictablesocial systems, or Complex Realism Sociology. Here, there is an agreement point. The conception of living systems as complex system as well as that of social system as complex systems acknowledge the autonomy of human reflexivity capability and free will be able to initiate the chain of events that triggers the process of adaptation to environment and change and social emergence ones, and, in so doing, problematize a

References

[1]  Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Wegner, D. (2004). On the Inference of Personal Authorship: Enhancing Experienced Agency by Priming Effect Information. Conscious Cognition, 14, 439-458.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.11.001
[2]  Archer, M. (2000). Being Human: The Problem of Agency. Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511488733
[3]  Archer, M. (2003). Structure, Agency, and the Internal Conversation. Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139087315
[4]  Archer, M. (2007). Making our Way through the World: Human Reflexivity and Social Mobility. Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618932
[5]  Arnason, G. (2011). Neuroscience, Free Will and Moral Responsibility. TRAMES-Journal of the Humanities and Social Sciences, 15, 147-155.
https://doi.org/10.3176/tr.2011.2.03
[6]  Aron, A. R., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S., Frank, M. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2007). Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3743-3752.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0519-07.2007
[7]  Badre, D. (2008). Cognitive Control, Hierarchy, and the Rostro-Caudal Organization of the Frontal Lobes. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 193-200.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004
[8]  Bailey, K. E. (1984). Beyond Functionalism: Toward a Nonequilibrium Analysis of Complex Social Systems. British Journal of Sociology, 35, 1-18.
https://doi.org/10.2307/590548
[9]  Bailey, K. E. (1994). Sociology & the New Systems Theory. Toward a Theoretical Synthesis. University of New York Press.
[10]  Banks, W. P., & Isham, E. A. (2009). We Infer Rather than Perceive the Moment We Decided to Act. Psychological Science, 20, 17-21.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02254.x
[11]  Bertelli, B., & Neresini, F. (1988). Complessità sociale, devianza e controllo: Alcune ipotesi teoriche di ridefinizione. Studi di Sociologia, 26, 43-63.
[12]  Berthoz, A. (2000). The Brain’s Sense of Movement. Harvard University Press.
[13]  Bode, S., & Haynes, J. D. (2009). Decoding Sequential Stages of Task Preparation in the Human Brain. NeuroImage, 4, 606-613.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.031
[14]  Bode, S., He, A. H., Soon, C. S., Trampel, R., Turner, R., & Haynes, J. D. (2011). Tracking the Unconscious Generation of Free Decisions Using Ultra-High Field fMRI. PLoS ONE, 6, e21612.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021612
[15]  Bode, S., Murawski, C., Soon, C. S., Bode, P., Stahl, J., & Smith P. L. (2014). Demystifying “Free Will”: The Role of Contextual Information and Evidence Accumulation for Predictive Brain Activity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Review, 47, 636-645.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.017
[16]  Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2007). To Do or Not to Do: The Neural Signature of Self-Control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 9141-9145.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0924-07.2007
[17]  Brown, J. W., Hanes, D. P., Schall, J. D., & Stuphorn, V. (2008). Relation of Frontal Eye Field Activity to Saccade Initiation during a Countermanding Task. Experimental Brain Research, 190, 135-151.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1455-0
[18]  Byrne, D. (1998). Complexity Theory & the Social Sciences: An Introduction. Routledge.
[19]  Byrne, D., & Callaghan, G. (2014). Complexity Theory & the Social Sciences: The State of the Art. Routledge.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203519585
[20]  Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, 200-219.
[21]  Changeux, P., & Ricoeur, P. (2000). What Makes Us Think? A Neuroscientist and a Philosopher Argue about Ethics, Human Nature, and the Brain. Princeton University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1515/9780691238265
[22]  Chiu, Y. C, & Yantis, S. (2009). A Domain-Independent Source of Cognitive Control for Task Sets: Shifting Spatial Attention and Switching Categorization Rules. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3930-3938.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5737-08.2009
[23]  Cilliers, P. (1998). Complexity & Postmodernism: Understanding Complex Systems. Routledge.
[24]  Cochran-Smith, M., Ell, F., Grudnoff, L., Ludlow, L., Haigh, M., & Hill, M. (2014). When Complexity Theory Meets Critical Realism: A Platform for Research on Initial Teacher Education. Teacher Education Quarterly, 41, 105-122.
[25]  Condorelli, R. (2016). Complex Systems Theory: Some Considerations for Sociology. Open Journal of Applied Sciences, 6, 422-448.
https://doi.org/10.4236/ojapps.2016.67044
[26]  de Cunha, R. V., & Relvas, J. B. (2017). Who’s Afraid of the Big Bad Neuroscience? Neuroscience’s Impact on Our Notions of Self and Free Will. In J. Leefmann, & E. Hildt (Eds.), The Human Sciences after the Decade of the Brain (pp. 24-41). Academic Press.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804205-2.00003-3
[27]  de Lafuente, V., Jazayeri, M., & Shadlen, M. N. (2015). Representation of Accumulating Evidence for a Decision in Two Parietal Areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 4306-4318.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2451-14.2015
[28]  Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Little Brown.
[29]  Desmurget, M., & Sirigu, A. (2009). A Parietal-Premotor Network for Movement Intention and Motor Awareness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13, 411-419.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.001
[30]  Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2008). A Dual-Networks Architecture of Top-Down Control. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 99-105.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001
[31]  Fried, I., Katz, A., McCarthy, G., Sass, K., Williamson, P., Spencer, S. S., & Spencer, D. D. (1991). Functional Organization of Human Supplementary Motor Cortex Studied by Electrical Stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 3656-3666.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-11-03656.1991
[32]  Fried, I., Mukamel, R., & Kreiman, G. (2011). Internally Generated Preactivation Single Neurons in Human Medial Frontal Cortex Predicts Volition. Neuron, 69, 548-562.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.045
[33]  Frith, C. D., & Haggard, P. (2018). Volition and the Brain—Revisiting a Classic Experimental Study. Trends in Neurosciences, 41, 405-407.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2018.04.009
[34]  Gallagher, S. (2006). Where’s the Action? Epiphenomenalism and the Problem of Free Will. In W. Banks, S. Pockett, & S. Gallagher (Eds.), Does Consciousness Cause Behavior? An Investigation in the Nature of Volition (pp. 109-124). The MIT Press.
https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262162371.003.0007
[35]  Gallagher, S., & Varela, F. J. (2003). Redrawing the Map and Resetting the Time: Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 29, 93-132.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00455091.2003.10717596
[36]  Gazzaniga, M. S. (2011). Neuroscience in the Courtroom. Scientific American, 304, 54-59.
https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0411-54
[37]  Gold, J. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2007). The Neural Basis of Decision Making. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 535-574.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038
[38]  Greene, J., & Cohen, J. (2004). For the Law, Neuroscience Changes Nothing and Everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 359, 1775-1785.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1546
[39]  Haggard, P. (2008). Human Volition: Towards a Neuroscience of Will. Nature Reviews of Neuroscience, 9, 934-946.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2497
[40]  Haggard, P. (2009). The Sources of Human Volition. Science, 324, 731-733.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1173827
[41]  Haggard, P. (2019). The Neurocognitive Bases of Human Volition. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 9-28.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103348
[42]  Haggard, P., & Eimer, M. (1999). On the Relation between Brain Potentials and the Awareness of Voluntary Movements. Experimental Brain Research, 126, 128-133.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210050722
[43]  Hallett, M. (2007). Volitional Control of Movement: The Physiology of Free Will. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 1179-1192.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.03.019
[44]  Hallett, M. (2016). Physiology of Free Will. Annals of Neurology, 80, 5-12.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24657
[45]  Harris, S. (2012). Free Will. Free Press.
[46]  Harvey, D. L., & Reed, M. (1997). Social Sciences as the Study of Complex Systems. In D. L. Kiel, & E. W. Elliott (Eds.), Chaos Theory in the Social Science: Foundations and Applications (pp. 295-323). The University of Michigan Press.
[47]  Haynes, J.-D. (2011). Beyond Libet. Long-Term Prediction of Free Choices from Neuroimaging Signals. In S. Dehaene, & Y. Cristen (Eds.), Characterizing Consciousness: From Cognition to the Clinic? Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences (pp. 161-175). Springer-Verlag.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-18015-6_10
[48]  Heekeren, H. R., Marrett, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2008). The Neural Systems That Mediate Human Perceptual Decision Making. Nature Reviews of Neuroscience, 9, 467-479.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2374
[49]  Heekeren, H. R., Marrett, S., Ruff, D. A., Bandettini, P. A., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2006). Involvement of Human Left Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Perceptual Decision Making Is Independent of Response Modality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 10023-10028.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0603949103
[50]  Jenkins, I. H., Jahanshahi, M., Jueptner, M., Passingham, R. E., & Brooks, D. J. (2000). Self-Initiated versus Externally Triggered Movements: II. The Effect of Movement Predictability on Regional Cerebral Blood Flow. Brain, 123, 1216-1228.
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.6.1216
[51]  Johnson, M. (1990). The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. University of Chicago Press.
[52]  Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Haggard, P., & Passingham, R. E. (2004a). Attention to Intention. Science, 303, 1208-1210.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1090973
[53]  Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Ramnani, N., & Passingham, R. E. (2004b). Willed Action and Attention to the Selection of Action. NeuroImage, 21, 1407-1415.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.034
[54]  Lavazza, A. (2016). Free Will and Neuroscience: From Explaining Freedom Away to New Ways of Operationalizing and Measuring It. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 1-29.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00262
[55]  Lavazza, A. (2019). Why Cognitive Sciences Do Not Prove That Free Will Is an Epiphenomenon. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 326.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00326
[56]  Lavazza, A., & Inglese, S. (2015). Operationalizing and Measuring (a Kind of) Free Will (and Responsibility). Towards a New Framework for Psychology, Ethics, and Law. Rivista Internazionale di Filosofia e Psicologia, 6, 37-55.
[57]  Legrenzi, P., & Umiltà, C. (2009). Neuro-mania. Il cervello non spiega chi siamo. Il Mulino.
[58]  Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious Cerebral Initiative and the Role of Conscious Will in Voluntary Action. Behavioral Brain Science, 8, 529-566.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00044903
[59]  Libet, B. (2009). Mind Time: The Temporal Factor in Consciousness. Harvard University Press.
[60]  Libet, B. (2011). Do We Have Free Will? In W. Sinnott-Armstrong, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Conscious Will and Responsibility: A Tribute to Benjamin Libet (pp. 1-10). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195381641.003.0002
[61]  Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of Conscious Intention to Act in Relation to Onset of Cerebral Activity (Readiness Potential): The Unconscious Initiation of a Freely Voluntary Act. Brain, 106, 623-642.
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623
[62]  Luhmann, N. (1984). Soziale Systeme. Suhrkamp.
[63]  Luhmann, N. (1986). The Autopoiesis of Social Systems. In F. Geyger, & J. van der Zowen (Eds.), Sociocybernetics Paradoxe (pp. 172-192). Sage.
[64]  Luhmann, N. (1990). Essays on Self Reference. Columbia University Press.
[65]  Luhmann, N., & De Giorgi, R. (1992). Teoria della società [Theory of Society]. Franco Angeli.
[66]  Maoz, U., & Yaffe, G. (2016). What Does Recent Neuroscience Tell Us about Criminal Responsibility? Journal of Law and the Biosciences, 3, 120-139.
https://doi.org/10.1093/jlb/lsv051
[67]  Maoz, U., Mudrik, L., Rivlin, R., Ross, I., Mamelak, A., & Yaffe, G. (2015). On Reporting the Onset of the Intention to Move. In A. R. Mele (Ed.), Surrounding Free Will: Philosophy, Psychology, Neuroscience (pp. 184-202). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199333950.003.0010
[68]  Maoz, U., Yaffe, G., Koch, C., & Mudrik, L. (2019). Neural Precursors of Decisions That Matter—An ERP Study of Deliberate and Arbitrary Choice. eLife, 8, e39787.
[69]  Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1972). De máquinas y seres vivos. Editorial Universitaria.
[70]  Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. D. Reidel Publishing Company.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-8947-4
[71]  Mele, A. (2014). Free. Why Science Hasn’t Disproved Free Will. Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199371624.001.0001
[72]  Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). La Phénoménologie de la Perception. Gallimard.
[73]  Moore, J. W., Lagnado, D., Deal, D. C., & Haggard, P. (2009). Feelings of Control: Contingency Determines Experience of Action. Cognition, 110, 279-283.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.006
[74]  Moore, J., & Haggard, P. (2008). Awareness of Action: Inference and Prediction. Conscious Cognition, 17, 136-144.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.12.004
[75]  Moore, M. S. (2011). Libet’s Challenge(s) to Responsible Agency. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Conscious Will and Responsibility: A Tribute to Benjamin Libet (pp. 207-234). Oxford University Press.
[76]  Morin, E. (1977). La Méthode. I. La nature de la nature [Method. Vol. I. Toward a Study of Human Kind]. Le Seuil.
[77]  Morin, E. (2008). On Complexity. Advances in Systems Theory, Complexity, and the Human Sciences. Hampton Press.
[78]  Mudrik, L., & Maoz, U. (2014). “Me & My Brain”: Exposing Neuroscience’s Closet Dualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 211-221.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00723
[79]  Nahmias, E., & Thompson, M., (2014). A Naturalistic Vision of Free Will. In E. O’Neill, & E. Machery (Eds.), Current Controversies in Experimental Philosophy (pp. 86-104). Routledge.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203122884-6
[80]  Nahmias, E., Shepard, J., & Reuter, S. (2014). It’s OK If ‘My Brain Made Me Do It’, People’s Intuitions about Free Will and Neuroscientific Prediction. Cognition, 133, 502-516.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.009
[81]  Nestor, P. G. (2019). In Defense of Free Will: Neuroscience and Criminal Responsibility. Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 65, Article ID: 101344.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2018.04.004
[82]  O’Hara, E. A. (2004). How Neuroscience Might Advance the law. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 359, 1677-1684.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1541
[83]  Palmer, J., Huk, A. C., & Shadlen, M. N. (2005). The Effect of Stimulus Strength on the Speed and Accuracy of a Perceptual Decision. Journal of Vision, 5, 376-404.
https://doi.org/10.1167/5.5.1
[84]  Perez, O., Mukamel, R., Tankus, A., Rosenblatt, J. D., Yeshurun, Y., & Fried I. (2015). Preconscious Prediction of a Driver’s Decision Using Intracranial Recordings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1492-1502.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00799
[85]  Petitot, J., Varela, F. J., Pachoud, B., & Roy J. M. (Eds.) (1999). Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. Stanford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1515/9781503617421
[86]  Praamstra, P., Boutsen, L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Frontoparietal Control of Spatial Attention and Motor Intention in Human EEG. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 764-774.
https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01052.2004
[87]  Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1979). La Nouvelle Alliance. Métamorphose de la Science. Gallimard.
[88]  Reed, M., & Harvey, D. L. (1992). The New Science and the Old: Complexity & Realism in the Social Sciences. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 22, 353-380.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1992.tb00224.x
[89]  Rosch, E. (2016). Introduction to the Revised Edition. In F. J. Varela, E. Thompson, & E. Rosch (Eds.), The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience (pp xxxv-lv). The MIT Press.
[90]  Roskies, A. L. (2010). How Does Neuroscience Affect Our Conception of Volition? The Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 109-130.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153151
[91]  Roskies, A. L. (2012). How Does the Neuroscience of Decision Making Bear on Our Understanding of Moral Responsibility and Free Will? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22, 1022-1026.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.009
[92]  Rossi, A. F., Pessoa, L., Desimone, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2009). The Prefrontal Cortex and the Executive Control of Attention. Experimental Brain Research, 192, 489-497.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1642-z
[93]  Salvaris, M., & Haggard, P. (2014). Decoding Intention at Sensorimotor Time Scales. PLoS ONE, 9, e85100.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0085100
[94]  Sawyer, K. R. (2005). Social Emergence. Societies as Complex Systems. Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511734892
[95]  Schlosser, M. E. (2015). The Neuroscience of Agency and Free Will. In C. W. Gruber, M. G. Clark, S. H. Klempe, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Constraints of Agency: Explorations of Theory in Everyday Life (pp. 113-124). Springer International Publishing.
[96]  Schurger, A. (2018). Specific Relationship between the Shape of the Readiness Potential, Subjective Decision Time, and Waiting Time Predicted by an Accumulator Model with Temporally Autocorrelated Input Noise. eNeuro, 5, ENEURO.0302-17.2018.
[97]  Schurger, A., Hu, P., Pak, J., & Roskies, A. L. (2021). What Is the Readiness Potential? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25, 558-570.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.04.001
[98]  Schurger, A., Mylopoulos, M., & Rosenthal, D. (2016). Neural Antecedents of Spontaneous Voluntary Movement: A New Perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 77-79.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.11.003
[99]  Schurger, A., Sitt, J. D., & Dehaene, S. (2012). An Accumulator Model Spontaneous Neural Activity Prior to Self-Initiated Movement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, E2904-E2913.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1210467109
[100]  Serences, J. T., & Yantis, S. (2007). Spatially Selective Representations of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Priority in Human Occipital, Parietal, and Frontal Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 284-293.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj146
[101]  Shibasaki, H., & Hallett, M. (2006). What Is the Bereitschaftspotential? Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 2341-2356.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.04.025
[102]  Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H. J., & Haynes, J. D. (2008). Unconscious Determinants of Free Decisions in the Human Brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 543-545.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2112
[103]  Soon, C. S., He, A. H., Bode, S., Haynes, J. D. (2013). Predicting Free Choices for Abstract Intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 5733-5734.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1212218110
[104]  Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind. Harvard University Press.
[105]  Trevena, J., & Miller, J. (2010). Brain Preparation before a Voluntary Action: Evidence against Unconscious Movement Initiation. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 447-456.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.006
[106]  Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology. A Methodological Remedy for the Hard Problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3, 330-349.
[107]  Varela, F. J., Thompson E., & Rosch E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. The MIT Press. (revised edition, Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (2016). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. The MIT Press).
https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001
[108]  Vierkant, T., Kiverstein, J., & Clark, A. (2013). Decomposing the Will: Meeting the Zombie Challenge. In A. Clark, J. Kiverstein, & T. Vierkant (Eds.), Decomposing the Will (pp. 1-30). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199746996.003.0001
[109]  von Foerster, H. (1960). On Self-Organizing Systems and Their Environments. In M. C. Yovitz, & S. Cameron (Eds.), Self-Organizing Systems (pp. 31-50). Pergamon Press.
[110]  Wallis, J. D. (2007). Orbitofrontal Cortex and Its Contribution to Decision Making. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 31-56.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094334
[111]  Waszak, F., Wascher, E., Keller, P., Koch, I., Aschersleben, G., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Prinz W. (2005). Intention-Based and Stimulus-Based Mechanisms in Action Selection. Experimental Brain Research, 162, 346-356.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2183-8
[112]  Wegner, D. M. (2002). The Illusion of Conscious Will. The MIT Press.
https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3650.001.0001
[113]  Wegner, D. M. (2003). The Mind’s Best Trick: How We Experience Conscious Will. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 65-69.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00002-0
[114]  Wegner, D. M. (2004). Précis of The Illusion of Conscious Will. Behavioural Brain Sciences, 27, 649-659.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000159
[115]  Williams, M. (2021). Realism and Complexity in Social Science. Routledge.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429443701
[116]  Williams, M., & Dyer, W. (2017). Complex Realism in Social Research. Methodological Innovations, 10, 1-8.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2059799116683564
[117]  Wisniewski, D., Goschke, T., & Haynes, J. D. (2016). Similar Coding of Freely Chosen and Externally Cued Intentions in a Fronto-Parietal Network. NeuroImage, 134, 450-458.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.044
[118]  Yang, Y. (2021). Critical Realism and Complexity Theory: Building a Nonconstructivist Systems Research Framework for Effective Governance Analysis. Systems Research and Behavioral Science, 38, 177-183.
https://doi.org/10.1002/sres.2662
[119]  Zeki, S., & Goodenough, O. (2006). Law and the Brain. Oxford University Press.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413